Part of
Bilingualism: A framework for understanding the mental lexicon
Edited by Maya Libben, Mira Goral and Gary Libben
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 6] 2017
► pp. 217248
References
Alladi, S., Bak, T. H., Duggirala, V., Surampudi, B., Shailaja, M., Shukla, A. K., Chaudhuri, J. R., & Kaul, S
(2013) Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology, 81, 1938–1944. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M
(1971) The control of short term memory. Scientific American, 225, 82–90. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H
(2007) languageR: Data sets and functions with “Analyzing Linguistic Data: A practical introduction to statistics”. R package version 0.4.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & Gulikers, L
(1995) The CELEX lexical database. Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Babcock, L., Stowe, J. C., Maloof, C. J., Brovetto, C., & Ullman, M. T
(2012) The storage and composition of inflected forms in adult-learned second language: A study of the influence of length of residence, age of arrival, sex, and other factors. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 820–840. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, D. M
(2005) lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.99875-9.Google Scholar
Beck, M.-L
(1997) Regular verbs, past tense and frequency: Tracking down a potential source of NS/NNS competence differences. Second Language Research, 13, 93–115. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beretta, A., Campbell, C., Carr, T. H., Huang, J., Schmitt, L. M., Christianson, K., & Cao, Y
(2003) An ER-fMRI investigation of morphological inflection in German reveals that the brain makes a distinction between regular and irregular forms. Brain and Language, 85, 67–92. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berres, M., Monsch, A. U., Bernasconi, F., Thalmann, B., & Stähelin, H. B
(2000) Normal ranges of neuropsychological tests for the diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease. Studies in Health Technology and Informatics, 77, 195–199.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E
(2009) Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 3–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Freedman, M
(2007) Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 45, 459–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G
(2008) Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 859–873. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Ryan, J
(2006) Executive control in a modified antisaccade task: Effects of aging and bilingualism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1341–1354. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K. F., & Yang, S
(2010) Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 525–531. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Martin, M. M., & Viswanathan, M
(2005) Bilingualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9, 103–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bittner, A
(1996) Starke 'schwache' Verben – schwache 'starke' Verben. Deutsche Verbflexion und Natürlichkeit. Tübingen: Stauffenburg.Google Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D
(2009) Praat: Doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 5.1. [URL] Google Scholar
Bowden, H. W., Gelfand, M. P., Sanz, C., & Ullman, M. T
(2010) Verbal inflectional morphology in L1 and L2 Spanish: A frequency effects study examining storage versus composition. Language Learning, 60, 44–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Buchweitz, A., & Prat, C
(2013) The bilingual brain: Flexibility and control in the human cortex. Physics of Life Reviews, 10, 428–443. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Buckner, R. L
(2004) Memory and executive function in aging and AD: Multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron, 44, 195–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burke, D. M., & MacKay, D. G
(1997) Memory, language, and ageing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 352, 1845–1856. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burke, D. M., & Shafto, M. A
(2008) Language and aging. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (pp. 373–443). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bybee, J
(1995) Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10, 425–455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, P. A., Miyake, A., & Just, M. A
(1994) Working memory constraints in comprehension: Evidence from individual difference, aphasia and aging. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 1075–1122). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Chandler, M. J., Lacritz, L. H., Hynan, L. S., Barnard, H. D., Allen, G., Deschner, M., Weiner, M. F., & Cullum, C. M
(2005) A total score for the CERAD neuropsychological battery. Neurology, 65, 102–106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, L., Shu, H., Liu, Y., Zhao, J., & Li, P
(2007) ERP signatures of subject-verb agreement in L2 learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10, 161–174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chertkow, H., Whitehead, V., Phillips, N., Wolfson, C., Atherton, J., & Bergman, H
(2010) Multilingualism (but not always bilingualism) delays the onset of Alzheimer disease: evidence from a bilingual community. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders, 24, 118–125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chialant, D., & Caramazza, A
(1995) Where is morphology and how is it processed? The case of written word recognition. In L. B. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 55–76). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H
(1997) The representation of participles in the German mental lexicon: Evidence for the dual mechanism processing. Yearbook of Morphology 1996, 73–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1999) Lexical entries and rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 991–1013. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C
(2006a) Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 3–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006b) How native-like is non-native language processing? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 564–570. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., Felser, C., Neubauer, K., Sato, M., & Silva, R
(2010) Morphological structure in native and nonnative language processing. Language Learning, 60, 21–43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Fleischhauer, E
(2014) Morphological priming in child German. Journal of Child Language, 41, 1305–1333. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., Hadler, M., & Weyerts, H
(2004) Speeded production of inflected words in children and adults. Journal of Child Language, 31, 683–712. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coughlin, C. E., & Tremblay, A
(2015) Morphological decomposition in native and non-native French speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 524–542. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Craik, F. I., Bialystok, E., & Freedman, M
(2010) Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease Bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology, 75, 1726–1729. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cross, E. S., & Burke, D. M
(2004) Do alternative names block young and older adults retrieval of proper names? Brain and Language, 89, 174–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davis, S. W., Zhuang, J., Wright, P., & Tyler, L. K
(2014) Age-related sensitivity to task-related modulation of language-processing networks. Neuropsychologia, 63, 107–115. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Drachman, D. A
(2006) Aging of the brain, entropy, and Alzheimer disease. Neurology, 67, 1340–1352. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duñabeitia, J. A., Marin, A., Aviles, A., Perea, M., & Carreiras, M
(2009) Constituent priming effects: Evidence for preserved morphological processing in healthy old readers. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21(2), 83–302.Google Scholar
Durrell, M
(2001) Strong verb ablaut in the West Germanic languages. In S. Watts, J. West, & H. Solms (Eds.), Zur Verbmorphologie germanischer Sprachen (pp. 5–18). Tübingen: Niemeyer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Embick, D., & Noyer, R
(2005) Distributed morphology and the syntax-morphology interface. In G. Ramchand & C. Reiss (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of linguistic interfaces (pp. 289–324). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ferreira, F., & Patson, N. D
(2007) The ‘good enough’approach to language comprehension. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1, 71–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Feyereisen, P
(1997) A meta-analytic procedure shows an age-related decline in picture naming: Comments on Goulet, Ska, and Kahn (1994). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 1328–1333. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fleischhauer, E., & Clahsen, H
(2012) Generating inflected word forms in real time: Evaluating the role of age, frequency, and working memory. In A. K. Biller, E. Y. Chung, & A. E. Kimball (Eds.), Proceedings of the 36th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 164–176). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R
(1975) “Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foote, R
(2011) Integrated knowledge of agreement in early and late English–Spanish bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 187–220. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C
(2003) DMDX: A windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behaviour Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 116–124. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Fennema-Notestine, C., Montoya, R. I., & Jernigan, T. L
(2007) The bilingual effect on Boston Naming Test performance. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 197–208.Google Scholar
Gollan, T., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T. C
(2008) More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 787–814. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Fennema-Notestine, C., & Morris, S. K
(2005) Bilingualism affects picture naming but not picture classification. Memory & Cognition, 33, 1220–1234. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., & Werner, G. A
(2002) Semantic and letter fluency in Spanish-English bilinguals. Neuropsychology, 16, 562–576. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greenwood, P. M
(2007) Functional plasticity in cognitive aging: Review and hypothesis. Neuropsychology, 21, 657–673. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H
(2006) Syntactic features and reanalysis in near-native processing. Second Language Research, 22, 369–397. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) Ultimate attainment in L2 inflection: Performance similarities between non-native and native speakers. Lingua, 120, 901–931. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Isaacs, B., & Kennie, A. T
(1973) The Set test as an aid to the detection of dementia in old people. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 123, 467–470. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackson, C. N., & Dussias, P. E
(2009) Cross-linguistic differences and their impact on L2 sentence processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 65–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jacob, G., Fleischhauer, E., & Clahsen, H
(2013) Allomorphy and affixation in morphological processing: A cross-modal priming study with late bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 924–933. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jiang, N
(2004) Morphological insensitivity in second language processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 603–634. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Selective integration of linguistic knowledge in adult second language learning. Language Learning, 57, 1–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, E. F., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S
(1983) The Boston naming test (2nd ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Lea & Febiger.Google Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M., & Marian, V
(2007) Bilingual language processing and interference in bilinguals: Evidence from eye tracking and picture naming. Language Learning, 57, 119–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kavé, G., & Levy, Y
(2005) The processing of morphology in old age: Evidence from Hebrew. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 1442–1451. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keating, G
(2009) Sensitivity to violations of gender agreement in native and nonnative Spanish: An eye-movement investigation. Language Learning, 59, 503–535. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kemper, S., Herman, R. E., & Lian, C. H
(2003) The costs of doing two things at once for young and older adults: Talking while walking, finger tapping, and ignoring speech of noise. Psychology and Aging, 18, 181–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Köpcke, K. M
(1998) The acquisition of plural marking in English and German revisited: schemata versus rules. Journal of Child Language, 25, 293–319. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lalleman, J. A., van Santen, A. J., & van Heuven, V. J
(1997) L2 processing of Dutch regular and irregular verbs. Review of Applied Linguistics, 115/116, 1–26.Google Scholar
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A
(2007) How first and second language learners use predictive cues in online sentence interpretation in Spanish and English. In H. Caunt-Nulton, S. Kulatilake, & I. Woo (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 382–393). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Light, L. L., & Anderson, P. A
(1985) Working-memory capacity, age, and memory for discourse. Journal of Gerontology, 40, 737–747. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacKay, D. G
(1987) The organization of perception and action: A theory for language and other cognitive skills. New York, NY: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacKay, D. G., & Burke, D. M
(1990) Chapter five cognition and aging: A theory of new learning and the use of old connections. Advances in Psychology, 71, 213–263. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maylor, E. A
(1998) Retrieving names in old age: Short- and (very) long-term effects of repetition. Memory & Cognition, 26, 309–319. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McDonald, J. L
(2006) Beyond the critical period: Processing-based explanations for poor grammaticality judgment performance by late second language learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 381–401. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McDonald, J. L., & Roussel, C. C
(2010) Past tense grammaticality judgment and production in non-native and stressed native English speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 429–448. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meinzer, M., Flaisch, T., Seeds, L., Harnish, S., Antonenko, D., Witte, V., Lindenberg, R., & Crosson, B
(2012a) Same modulation but different starting points: Performance modulates age differences in inferior frontal cortex activity during word-retrieval. PLoS One, 7, e33631. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meinzer, M., Seeds, L., Flaisch, T., Harnish, S., Cohen, M. L., McGregor, K., Conway, T., Benjamin, M., & Crosson, B
(2012b) Impact of changed positive and negative task-related brain activity on word-retrieval in aging. Neurobiology of Aging, 33, 656–669. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Neubauer, K., & Clahsen, H
(2009) Decomposition of inflected words in a second language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 31, 403–435. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oller, D. K., Pearson, B. Z., & Cobo-Lewis, A. B
(2007) Profile effects in early bilingual language and literacy. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 191–230. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Park, D. C
(2012) The basic mechanisms accounting for age-related decline in cognitive function. In D. C. Park & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Cognitive aging: A primer (pp. 3–21). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Penke, M., & Westermann, G
(2006) Broca's area and inflectional morphology: Evidence from Broca's aphasia and computer modeling. Cortex, 42, 563–576. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S
(1999) Words and rules. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Pinker, S., & Ullman, M. T
(2002) The past and future of the past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 456–463. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prado, E. L., & Ullman, M. T
(2009) Can imageability help us draw the line between storage and composition? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 849–866. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prasada, S., Pinker, S., & Snyder, W
(1990) Some evidence that irregular forms are retrieved from memory but regular forms are rule-governed. Paper presented at the 31st meeting of the Psychonomic Society, New Orleans, LA.Google Scholar
Rastle, K. G., & Burke, D. M
(1996) Priming the tip of the tongue: Effects of prior processing on word retrieval in young and older adults. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 586–605. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rosen, W. G., Mohs, R. C., & Davis, K. L
(1984) A new rating scale for Alzheimer's disease. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 1356–1364. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sagarra, N., & Herschensohn, J
(2010) The role of proficiency and working memory in gender and number agreement processing in L1 and L2 Spanish. Lingua, 120, 2022–2039. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A
(1996) The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–428. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2000) Aging and measures of processing speed. Biological Psychology, 54, 35–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) Selective review of cognitive aging. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16, 754–760. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A., Atkinson, T. M., & Berish, D. E
(2003) Executive functioning as a potential mediator of age-related cognitive decline in normal adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 566–594. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sato, M., & Felser, C
(2010) Sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations in English as a second language. Second Language, 9, 101–118.Google Scholar
Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H
(1995) Modeling morphological processing. In L. B. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 131–154). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S., & Bruck, M
(1990, November). Consistency effects in the generation of past tense morphology. Paper presented at the 31st meeting of the Psychonomic Society, New Orleans, LA.
Sereno, J. A., & Jongman, A
(1997) Processing of English inflectional morphology. Memory & Cognition, 25, 425–437. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shafto, M. A., Burke, D. M., Stamatakis, E. A., Tam, P. P., & Tyler, L. K
(2007) On the tip-of-the-tongue: Neural correlates of increased word-finding failures in normal aging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 2060–2070. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shenkman, K. D
(1994) Structure sensitivity and language processing in adult learners of English. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Rochester.Google Scholar
Silva, R., & Clahsen, H
(2008) Morphologically complex words in L1 and L2 processing: Evidence from masked priming experiments in English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 245–260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smolka, E., Zwitserlood, P., & Rösler, F
(2007) Stem access in regular and irregular inflection: Evidence from German participles. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 325–347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sonnenstuhl, I., Eisenbeiss, S., & Clahsen, H
(1999) Morphological priming in the German mental lexicon. Cognition, 72, 203–236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Szagun, G
(2011) Regular/irregular is not the whole story: The role of frequency and generalization in the acquisition of German past participle inflection. Journal of Child Language, 38, 731–762. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tanner, D., Osterhout, L., & Herschensohn, J
(2009) Snapshots of grammaticalization: Differential electrophysiological responses to grammatical anomalies with increasing L2 exposure. In J. Chandlee, M. Franchini, S. Lord, & G. -M. Rheiner (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 528–539). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Tewes, U
(1991) HAWIE-R.Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligenztest für Erwachsene. Revision 1991. Bern: Huber.Google Scholar
Tyler, L. K., Shafto, M. A., Randall, B., Wright, P., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Stamatakis, E. A
(2010) Preserving syntactic processing across the adult life span: The modulation of the frontotemporal language system in the context of age-related atrophy. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 352–364. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ullman, M. T
(1993) The computation of inflectional morphology. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
(2005) A cognitive neuroscience perspective on second language acquisition: The declarative/procedural model. In C. Sanz (Ed.), Mind and context in adult second language acquisition: Methods, theory, and practice (pp. 141–178). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Valian, V
(2015) Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 3–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Verhaeghen, P., Marcoen, A., & Goosens, L
(1993) Facts and fiction about memory aging: A quantitative integration of research findings. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 48, 157–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Verhaeghen, P., & Salthouse, T. A
(1997) Meta-analyses of age-cognition relations in adulthood: estimates of linear and nonlinear age effects and structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 231–249. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Veríssimo, J., Heyer, V., Jacob, G., & Clahsen, H
(2017) Selective effects of age of acquisition on morphological priming: Evidence for a sensitive period. Language Acquisition.Advance online publication. DOI logo
Voga, M., Anastassiadis-Symeonidis, A., & Giraudo, H
(2014) Does morphology play a role in L2 processing? Two masked priming experiments with Greek speakers of ESL. Lingvisticæ Investigationes, 37, 338–352.Google Scholar
Waters, G., & Caplan, D
(2001) Age, working memory and on-line syntactic processing in sentence comprehension. Psychology and Aging, 16, 128–144. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Waters, G. S., & Caplan, D
(2005) The relationship between age, processing speed, working memory capacity, and language comprehension. Memory, 13, 403–413. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Westermann, G., Kovic, V., & Ruh, N
(2008) Mechanisms of verb inflections-Regular vs. irregular or easy vs. hard. In Proceedings of the 30th Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 739–744). Austin, TX.
Weyerts, H., & Clahsen, H
(1994) Netzwerke und symbolische Regeln im Spracherwerb: experimentelle Ergebnisse zur Entwicklung der Flexionsmorphologie. Linguistische Berichte, 154, 430–460.Google Scholar
Wunderlich, D
(1996) Minimalist morphology: The role of paradigms. Yearbook of Morphology 1995, 93–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wunderlich, D., & Fabri, R
(1995) Minimalist morphology: An approach to inflection. Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft, 14, 236–294. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yaden, B
(2007) The processing and representation of verbal inflection: Data from L1 and L2 Spanish. Hispania, 90, 795–808. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 7 other publications

Dominguez, Alberto, Anthea Santos & Yang Fu
2023. The Access to Grammatical Number in Spanish Children and Adults. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 52:6  pp. 2499 ff. DOI logo
Hardy, Sophie M., Katrien Segaert & Linda Wheeldon
2020. Healthy Aging and Sentence Production: Disrupted Lexical Access in the Context of Intact Syntactic Planning. Frontiers in Psychology 11 DOI logo
Jessen, Anna, João Veríssimo & Harald Clahsen
2018. Variability and consistency in late bilinguals’ morphology. The Mental Lexicon 13:2  pp. 186 ff. DOI logo
Reifegerste, Jana
2021. The effects of aging on bilingual language: What changes, what doesn't, and why. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 24:1  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
REIFEGERSTE, JANA, KIRILL ELIN & HARALD CLAHSEN
2019. Persistent differences between native speakers and late bilinguals: Evidence from inflectional and derivational processing in older speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 22:3  pp. 425 ff. DOI logo
Royle, Phaedra, Karsten Steinhauer, Émie Dessureault, Alexandre C. Herbay & Simona M. Brambati
2019. Aging and Language: Maintenance of Morphological Representations in Older Adults. Frontiers in Communication 4 DOI logo
Savinova, Elena & Svetlana Malyutina
2021. Evidence for dual-route morphological processing across the lifespan: data from Russian noun plurals. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 36:6  pp. 730 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 22 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.