References (55)
References
Aylett, M., & A. E. Turk. (2004). The smooth signal redundancy hypothesis: A functional explanation for relationships between redundancy, prosodic prominence, and duration in spontaneous speech. Language and Speech, 47, 31–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bell, A., Brenier, J. M., Gregory, M., Girand, C., & Jurafsky, D. (2009). Predictability effects on durations of content and function words in conversational English. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 92–111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boas, H. C. (to appear). Finding constructions and their networks: Some proposals for constructicography and its contributions to Construction Grammar. In A. Ziem et al. (Eds.), Advances in Constructicography.
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. J. M. (2019). Praat: doing phonetics by computer (Version 6.1.08). Downloaded 5. 12. 2019, [URL]
Boulenger, V., Hoen, M., Jacquier, C., & Meunier, F. (2011). Interplay between acoustic/phonetic and semantic processes during spoken sentence comprehension: An ERP study. Brain and Language, 116(2), 51–63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brand, S., & Ernestus, M. (2019). In S. Calhoun, P. Escudero, M. Tabain, & P. Warren (Eds.). Proceedings of the 19th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Melbourne, Australia 2019 (pp. 97–101). Canberra.Google Scholar
Brenner, D., Warner, N., Ernestus, M., & Tucker, B. V. (2011). Parsing the ambiguity of casual speech: “He was like” or “He’s like”? The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 129, 2683. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brouwer, S., Mitterer, H., & Huettig, F. (2012a). Speech reductions change the dynamics of competition during spoken word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27(4), 539–571. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012b). Can hearing puter activate pupil? Phonological competition and the processing of reduced spoken words in spontaneous conversations. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(11), 2193–2220. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, G., & Yule, G. (1983). Discourse analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. (2007). Frequency of use and the organization of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010). Language, usage and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J., File-Muriel, R., & Napoleão de Souza, R. (2016). Special reduction: A usage-based approach. Language and Cognition, 8(3), 421–446. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J., & Scheibman, J. (1999). The effect of usage on degrees of constituency: The reduction of don’t in English. Linguistics: An Interdisciplinary Journal of the Language Sciences, 37(4), 575–596. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dankovičová, J. (1997). The domain of articulation rate variation in Czech. Journal of Phonetics, 25, 287–312. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ernestus, M., & Baayen, R. H. (2007). The comprehension of acoustically reduced morphologically complex words: The roles of deletion, duration, and frequency of occurrence. In Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 773–776). Saarbrücken.Google Scholar
Ernestus, M., Baayen, R. H., & Schreuder, R. (2002). The recognition of reduced word forms. Brain and Language, 81, 162–173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fried, M. (1994). Second-position clitics in Czech: Syntactic or phonological? Lingua, 94, 155–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003). Word order. In Handbook of Pragmatics, 1–13. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fried, M., & Machač, P. (2022). Intonation as a cue to epistemic stance in one type of insubordinate clauses. Folia Linguistica, 56(1), 183–214. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fried, M., & Östman, J-O. (2004). Construction Grammar: A thumbnail sketch. In M. Fried, & J-O. Östman (Eds.), Construction Grammar in a cross-language perspective (pp. 11–86). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gahl, S., Yao, Y., & Johnson, K. (2012). Why reduce? Phonological neighborhood density and phonetic reduction in spontaneous speech. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 789–806. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, S. (1999). Speaking in shorthand — a syllable-centric perspective for understanding pronunciation variation. Speech Communication, 29, 159–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Janse, E., & Ernestus, M. (2011). The roles of bottom-up and top-down information in the recognition of reduced speech: Evidence from listeners with normal and impaired hearing. Journal of Phonetics, 39(3), 330–343. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, K. (2004). Massive reduction in conversational American English. Proceedings of the 10th international symposium on spontaneous speech: Data and analysis (pp. 29–54). Tokyo, Japan.Google Scholar
Jurafsky, D., Bell, A., Gregory, M., & Raymond, W. (2001). Probabilistic relations between words: Evidence from reduction in lexical production. In J. Bybee, & P. Hopper (Eds.), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure (pp. 229–254). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keune, K., Ernestus, M., van Hout, R., & Baayen, H. (2005). Social, geographical, and register variation in Dutch: From written mogelijk to spoken mok. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 1, 183–223. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kohler, K. J. (1990). Segmental reduction in connected speech in German: Phonological facts and phonetic explanations. In W. J. Hardcastle, & A. Marchal (Eds.), Speech production and speech modelling (pp. 21–33). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kohler, K. J., & Niebuhr, O. (2011). On the role of articulatory prosodies in German message decoding. Phonetica, 68(1–2), 57–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kopřivová, M., Komrsková, Z., Lukeš, D., Poukarová, P., & Škarpová, M. (2017). ORTOFON: Korpus neformální mluvené češtiny s víceúrovňovým přepisem [Corpus of informal spoken Czech in multilayered transcription]. Praha: Ústav Českého národního korpusu FF UK.Google Scholar
Kvale, K., & Foldvik, A. K. (1991). Manual segmentation and labelling of continuous speech. In: ESCA Workshop on Phonetics and Phonology of Speaking Styles: Reduction and Elaboration in Speech Communication (paper 37). Barcelona.Google Scholar
Labov, W. L. (2001). Principles of linguistic change: Social factors. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Lambrecht, K. (1994). Information structure and sentence form: Topic, focus, and the mental representations of discourse referents. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lindblom, B. (1990). Explaining phonetic variation: A sketch of the H&H theory. In M. Hardcastle (Ed.), Speech production and speech modeling (pp. 403–440). Dordrecht: Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Machač, P., & Fried, M. (2021). Fonetické redukce a vyjadřování jistotních postojů v insubordinačních strukturách [Phonetic reductions and epistemic marking in insubordination structures]. Časopis pro moderní filologii, 103(1), 7–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Machač, P., & Skarnitzl, R. (2009). Principles of phonetic segmentation. Praha: Nakladatelství Epocha.Google Scholar
(2020). Stability of phonetic features of Czech plosives in spontaneous speech. Studie z aplikované lingvistiky, 12(2), 16–36.Google Scholar
Machač, P., & Zíková, M. (2013). Redukční procesy v řeči z hlediska fonetických rysů [Reduction processes in the light of phonetic features]. In: O. Uličný, & M. Prošek (Eds.), Studie k moderní mluvnici češtiny 5, K české fonetice a pravopisu (pp. 17–44). Olomouc: Univerzita Palackého.Google Scholar
(2015). Parallel articulation: The phonetic base and the phonological potentiality. Slovo a slovesnost, 76(1), 3–21.Google Scholar
Méli, A., Ballier, N., Falaise, A., & Henderson, A. (2021). An experiment in paratone detection in a prosodically annotated EAP Spoken Corpus. Proeedings of Interspeech 2021, ISCA (pp.2616–2620), ff10.21437/Interspeech.2021-294ff. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mitterer, H., & Ernestus, M. (2006). Listeners recover /t/s that speakers reduce: Evidence from /t/-lenition in Dutch. Journal of Phonetics, 34(1), 73–103. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mitterer, H., & Tuinman, A. (2012). The role of native-language knowledge in the perception of casual speech in a second language. Frontiers in psychology, 3, 249. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Niebuhr, O., & Kohler, K. J. (2011). Perception of phonetic detail in the identification of highly reduced words. Journal of Phonetics, 39, 319–329. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Östman, J-O. (2005). Construction discourse: A prolegomenon. In J-O. Östman, & M. Fried (Eds.), Construction grammars: Cognitive grounding and theoretical extensions (pp. 121–144). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015). From Construction Grammar to Construction Discourse … and back. In J. Bücker, S. Günthner, & W. Imo (Eds.), Konstruktionsgrammatik V. Konstruktionen im Spannungsfeld von sequenziellen Mustern, kommunikativen Gattungen und Textsorten (pp. 15–43). Tübingen: Stauffenburg.Google Scholar
Phillips, B. S. (1994). Southern English glide deletion revisited. American Speech, 69, 15–127. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pluymakers, M., Ernestus, M., & Baayen, H. (2005a). Lexical frequency and acoustic reduction in spoken Dutch. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 118, 2561–2569. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005b). Articulatory planning is continuous and sensitive to informational redundancy. Phonetica, 62, 146–159. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Raymond, W. D., Dautricourt, R., & Hume, E. (2006). Word-internal /t,d/ deletion in spontaneous speech: Modeling the effects of extra-linguistic, lexical, and phonological factors. Language Variation and Change, 18, 55–97. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sivonen, P., Maess, B., Lattner, S., & Friederici, A. D. (2006). Phonemic restoration in a sentence context: Evidence from early and late ERP effects. Brain Research, 121(1), 177–189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Strik, H., van Doremalen, J., & Cucchiarini, C. (2008). Pronunciation reduction: How it relates to speech style, gender, and age. Proceedings of Interspeech (pp. 1477–1480). Brisbane: ISCA. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tucker, B. V., & Ernestus, M. (2016). Why we need to investigate casual speech to truly understand language production, processing and the mental lexicon. The mental lexicon, 11(3), 375–400. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Bael, C., Baayen, H., & Strik, H. (2007). Segment deletion in spontaneous speech: A corpus study using mixed effects models with crossed random effects. Proceedings of Interspeech 2007 (pp. 2741–2744). Antwerp, Belgium. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Warner, N., Brenner, D., Woods, A., Tucker, B. V., & Ernestus, M. (2009). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125(4), 2655. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zíková, M., & Machač, P. (2014). Variability in phonetic realization of the demonstrative ten in terms of its informational relevance in the sentence. In: L. Veselovská, & M. Janebová (Eds.), Nominal structures: All in complex DPs (pp. 98–109). Olomouc: Univerzita Palackého.Google Scholar