Review published In:
Consciousness & Emotion
Vol. 2:2 (2001) ► pp.321329
References
Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, A. U.
(1999) A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 1061, 529–550. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chalmers, D. J.
(1996) The conscious mind: In search of a fundamental theory. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. S.
(1998) Feeling reasons. In P. M. Churchland & P. S. Churchland (Eds.), On the contrary: Critical essays, 1987–1997 (pp. 231–254). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Critchley, H. D., Elliot, R., Mathias, C. J., & Dolan, R. J.
(2000) Neural activity relating to generation and representation of galvanic skin conductance responses: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. The Journal of Neuroscience, 201, 3033–3040. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Damasio, A. R.
(1994) Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. New York: G.P. Putnam.Google Scholar
(1999) The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace and Company.Google Scholar
Ellis, R. D., & Newton, N.
(Eds.)(2000) The caldron of consciousness: Motivation, affect and self-organization. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Elster, J.
(1999) Strong feelings: Emotion, addiction, and human behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Farthing, G. W.
(1992) The psychology of consciousness. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Frith, C. D.
(1992) The cognitive neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Frith, C. D., & Frith, U.
(1999) Interacting minds – A biological basis. Science, 2861, 1692–1695. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lane, R. D.
(2000) Neural correlates of conscious emotional experience. In R. D. Lane, L. Nadel, G. L. Ahern, J. J. B. Allen, A. W. Kaszniak, S. Z. Rapcsak, & G. E. Schwartz (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 345–370). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lane, R. D., Nadel, L., Allen, J. J. B., & Kaszniak, A. W.
(2000) The study of emotion from the perspective of cognitive neuroscience. In R. D. Lane, L. Nadel, G. L. Ahern, J. J. B. Allen, A. W. Kaszniak, S. Z. Rapcsak, & G. E. Schwartz (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 3–11). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J.
(1996) The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Öhman, A., & Wiens, S.
(2001) To think and to feel: Nonconscious emotional activation and consciousness. In A. W. Kaszniak (Ed.), Emotions, qualia, and consciousness (pp. 363–385). Singapore: World Scientific. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, J.
(1998) Affective neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Schwartz, N., & Clore, G. L.
(1996) Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In E. T. Higgins & A. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 433–465). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Solomon, R. C.
(2000) The philosophy of emotions. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 3–15). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Stocker, M.
(1996) Valuing emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Watt, D. F.
(1999) At the intersection of emotion and consciousness: Affective neuroscience and extended reticular thalamic activating system (ERTAS) theories of consciousness. In S. R. Hameroff, A. W. Kaszniak, & D. J. Chalmers (Eds.), Toward a science of consciousness III: The third Tucson discussions and debates (pp. 215–229). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2001) Affective neuroscience and extended reticular thalamic activating system (ERTAS) theories of consciousness. In A. W. Kaszniak (Ed.), Emotions, qualia, and consciousness (pp. 290–320). Singapore: World Scientific. DOI logoGoogle Scholar