References (80)
References
Ajuriaguerra, Julián de. 1962. “Le corps comme relation.” Revue neuropsychiatrie infantile 21: 137–154.Google Scholar
Apter, Gisèle and Candilis, Drina. 2005. “A challenge for perinatal psychiatry: Therapeutic management of maternal borderline personality disorder and their very young infants.” Clinical Neuropsychiatry 2 (5): 302–314.Google Scholar
Apter, Gisèle, Devouche, Emmanuel, Garez, Valérie, Valente, Marina, Genet, Marie-Camille, Gratier, Maya, Dominguez, Sara and Tronick, Edward. in press. “The Still face: A greater challenge for infants of mothers with borderline personality disorder.” Journal of Personality Disorders.
Bazhenova, Olga V., Oxana, Plonskaia and Porges, Stephen W. 2001. “Vagal reactivity and affective adjustment in infants during interaction challenges.” Child Development 72 (5): 1314–1326. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergeson, Tonya R. and Trehub, Sandra E. 2007. “Signature tunes in mothers’ speech to infants.” Infant Behavior and Development 30: 648–654. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brazelton, T. Berry, Koslowski, Barbara and Main, Mary. 1974. “The origins of reciprocity: The early mother-infant interaction.” In The Effect of the Infant On Its Caregiver, Michael Lewis and Leonard A. Rosenblum (eds), 49–76. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Charrier, Isabelle, Mathevon, Nicolas and Jouventin, Pierre. 2003. “Vocal signature recognition of mother by fur seal pups.” Animal Behaviour 65: 543–550. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eric F. 1989. “The perception of expressive timing in music.” Psychological Research 51: 2–9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, Herbert H. 1989. “Contributing to discourse.” Cognitive Science 13: 259–294. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, Herbert H. and Brennan, Susan A. 1991. “Grounding in communication.” In Perspectives On Socially Shared Cognition, Lauren B. Resnick, John M. Levine and Stephanie D. Teasley (eds), 127–149. Washington: American Psychological Association. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cooper, Robin P. and Aslin, Richard N. 1990. “Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth.” Child Development 61: 1584–1595. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cowley, Stephen J., Moodley, Sheshni and Fiori-Cowley, Agnese. 2004. “Grounding signs of culture: Primary intersubjectivity and social semiosis.” Mind , Culture and Activity 11 (2): 109–132. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crandell, Lisa E., Patrick, Matthew P.H. and Hobson, Peter. 2003. “Still-face interactions between mothers with borderline personality disorder and their 2-month-old infants.” British Journal of Psychiatry 183: 239–247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
D’Odorico, Laura and Franco, Fabia. 1991. “Selective production of vocalization types in different communication contexts.” Journal of Child Language 18: 475–499. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Casper, Anthony J. and Fifer, William P. 1980. “Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mother’s voices.” Science 208 (4448): 1174–1176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Jaegher, Hanne and Di Paolo, Ezekiel A. 2007. “Participatory sense-making: An enactive approach to social cognition.” Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 6: 485–507. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Delack, J.B. and Fowlow, P.J. 1978. “The ontogenesis of differential vocalizations: Development of prosodic contrastivity during the first year of life.” In The Development of Communication, Natalie Waterson and Catherine Snow (eds), 93–110. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Delavenne, Anne, Gratier, Maya, Devouche, Emmanuel and Apter, Gisèle. 2008. “Phrasing and fragmented time in ‘pathological’ mother-infant vocal interaction.” Musicae Scientiae 12 (1): 47–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Devouche, Emmanuel and Gratier, Maya. 2001. “Microanalyse du rythme dans les échanges vocaux et gestuels entre la mère et son bébé de 10 semaines.” Devenir 13 (2): 55–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernald, Anne. 1985. “Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese.” Infant Behavior and Development 8: 181–195. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernald, Anne, Taeschner, Traute, Dunn, Judy, Papoušek, Mechthild, de Boisson-Bardies, Bénédicte and Fukui, Ikuko. 1989. “A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants.” Journal of Child Development 16: 477–501.Google Scholar
Filippa, Manuela, Gratier, Maya, Devouche, Emmanuel and Grandjean, Didier. submitted. “Premature newborns’ behavior modulates affect in mothers’ voice during infant-directed communication: Evidence for early social interaction.”
Fogel, Alan. 1977. “Temporal organization in mother-infant face-to-face interaction.” In Studies in Mother-infant Interaction, H. Rudolph Schaeffer (ed.), 119–151. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gergely, Gyorgy and Watson, John S. 1999. “Early social-emotional development: Contingency perception and the social biofeedback model.” In Early Social Cognition, Philippe Rochat (ed.), 101–136. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gratier, Maya. 2003. “Expressive timing and interactional synchrony between mothers and infants: Cultural similarities, cultural differences, and the immigration experience.” Cognitive Development 18: 533–554. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2008. “Grounding in musical communication: Evidence from jazz duet performance.” Musicae Scientiae 12 (1): 71–110. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2010. “La dimension tacite de l’interaction sociale: communication mère-bébé et improvisation musicale”. Unpublished Manuscript. Université Paris Ouest Nanterre La Défense.Google Scholar
Gratier, Maya and Apter-Danon, Gisèle. 2009. “The musicality of belonging: Repetition and variation in mother-infant interaction.” In Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship, Colwyn Trevarthen and Stephen Malloch (eds), 245–263. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gratier, Maya and Devouche, Emmanuel. 2011. “Imitation and repetition of prosodic contour in vocal interaction at 3 months.” Developmental Psychology 47: 67–76. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gratier, Maya and Magnier, Julien. 2012. “Sense and synchrony: Infant communication and musical improvisation.” Intermédialités 19: 45–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gratier, Maya and Trevarthen, Colwyn. 2008. “Musical narrative and motives for culture in mother-infant vocal interaction.” The Journal of Consciousness Studies 15 (10–11): 122–158.Google Scholar
Guellaï, Bahia and Streri, Arlette. 2011. “Cues for early social skills: Direct gaze modulates newborns’ recognition of talking faces.” PLoS ONE 6 (4): e18610. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huron, David. 2006. Sweet Anticipation. Music and the Psychology of Expectation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jaffe, Joseph, Beebe, Beatrice, Feldstein, Stanley, Crown, Cynthia L. and Jasnow, Michael D. 2001. “Rhythms of dialogue in infancy.” Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 66 (2): 1–148. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Janik, Vincent M. and Slater, Peter J.B. 1998. “Context-specific use suggests that bottlenose dolphin signature whistles are cohesion calls.” Animal Behavior 56: 829–838. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jeanneau, Madeleine and Armelius, Bengt-Åke. 1993. “Linguistic characteristics of neurotic, borderline and psychotic personality organization.” Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 34 (1): 64–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keller, Heidi and Scholmerich, Axel. 1987. “Infant vocalizations and parental reactions during the first 4 months of life.” Developmental Psychology 23 (1): 62–67. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kessen, William, Janice Levine and Kenneth A. Wendrich. 1979. “The imitation of pitch in infants.” Infant Behavior and Development 2: 93–99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kugiumutzakis, Giannis. 1993. “Intersubjective vocal imitation in early mother-infant interaction.” In New Perspectives in Early Communicative Development, Jacqueline Nadel and Luigia Camaioni (eds), 23–47. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Lamour, Martine and Barraco, Marthe. 1998. Souffrances Autour du Berceau-Des Emotions au Soin. Gaëtan. Levallois-Perret, France: Morin.Google Scholar
Langer, Suzanne. 1942. Philosophy in a New Key: A Study in the Symbolism of Reason, Rite and Art. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Legerstee, Maria. 1990. “Infants use multimodal stimulation to imitate speech sounds.” Infant Behavior and Development 13: 345–356. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, Maria, Corter, Carl and Kineapple, Kim. 1998. “Hand, arm and facial actions of young infants to a social and nonsocial stimulus.” Child Development 61: 774–784. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Malloch, Stephen. 1999. “Mothers and infants and communicative musicality.” Musicae Scientiae Special Issue: Rhythm, Musical Narrative, and the Origins of Human Communication: 29–58.Google Scholar
Malloch, Stephen and Trevarthen, Colwyn. 2009. Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mampe, Birgit, Friederici, Angela D., Christophe, Anne and Wermke, Kathleen. 2009. “Newborns’ cry melody is shaped by their native language.” Current Biology 19 (23): 1994–1997. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meyer, Leonard. 1956. Emotion and Meaning in Music. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Monson, Ingrid. 1996. Saying Something: Jazz Improvisation and Interaction. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Montgomery, Stuart A. and Asberg, Marie. 1979. “A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change.” British Journal of Psychiatry 134: 382–389. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moon, Christine, Lagercrantz, Hugo and Kuhl, Patricia K. 2013. “Language experienced in utero affects vowel perception after birth: A two-country study.” Acta Paedritica 102 (2): 156–160. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moore, Ginger A. 2010. “Parent conflict predicts infant’s vagal regulation in social interaction.” Development and Psychopathology 22: 23–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moore, Ginger A., Hill-Soderlund, Ashley L., Propper, Cathy B., Calkins, Susan D., Mills-Koonce, W. Roger and Cox, Martha J. 2009. “Mother-infant vagal regulation in the face-to-face still-face paradigm is moderated by maternal sensitivity.” Child Development 80: 209–223. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Murray, Lynn and Trevarthen, Colwyn T. 1985. “Emotional regulation of interactions between two‑month‑olds and their mothers.” In Social Perception in Infants, Tiffany M. Field and Nathan A. Fox (eds), 177–197. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Nadel, Jacqueline, Carchon, Isabelle, Marcelli, Daniel and Réserbat-Plantey, Denis. 1999. “Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds.” Developmental Science 2 (2): 164–173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oller, D. Kimbrough. 1980. “The emergence of the sounds of speech in infancy.” In Child Phonology, G. Yeni-Komshian, J. Kavanaugh, and C. Fergusson (eds), 93–122. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
. 2000. The Emergence of the Speech Capacity. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Papoušek, Mechthild and Papoušek, Michael. 1989. “Forms and functions of vocal matching in interactions between mothers and their pre-canonical infants.” First language 9: 137–158. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Patel, Aniruddh D., Iversen, John R., Bregman, Micah R. and Schulz, Irena. 2009. “Experimental evidence for synchronization to a musical beat in nonhuman animals.” Current Biology 19: 827–830. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pfohl, Bruce B. and Zimmerman, Marc. 1995. Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality (SIDP-IV). Iowa City, IA: University of Iowa.Google Scholar
Reddy, Vasudevi, Hay, David, Murray, Lynne and Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1997. “Communication in infancy: Mutual regulation of affect and attention.” In Infant Development: Recent Advances, Gavin Bremner, Alan Slater and George Butterworth (eds), 247–274. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, Robert M. and Cheney, Dorothy L. 2003. “Signalers and receivers in animal communication.” Annual Review of Psychology 54: 145–173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spitz, René. 1965. Vom Säugling zum Kleinkind. Naturgeschichte der Mutter-Kind-Beziehungen im ersten Lebensjahr. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta.Google Scholar
Stern, Daniel N. 1977. The First Relationship: Infant and Mother. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
. 1985. The Interpersonal World of the Infant: A View from Psychoanalysis and Developmental Psychology. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
. 2010. Forms of Vitality: Exploring Dynamic Experience in Psychology, the Arts, Psychotherapy, and Development. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stern, Daniel N., Jaffe, Joseph, Beebe, Beatrice and Bennett, S.L. 1975. “The infant’s stimulus world during social interaction: A study of caregiver behaviours with particular reference to repetition and timing.” In Studies in Mother-Infant Interaction, Rudolph Schaeffer (ed.), 177–202. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Takahashi, Daniel Y., Narayanan, Darshana Z. and Ghazanfar, Asif A. 2013. “Coupled oscillator dynamics of vocal turn-taking in monkeys.” Current Biology 23 (21): 2162–2168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael. 1999. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, Kruger, Ann Cale and Ratner, Hillary H. 1993. “Cultural learning.” Behavioural and Brain Sciences 16 (3): 495–552. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trehub, Sandra E. and Thorpe, Leigh A. 1989. “Infants’ perception of rhythm: Categorisation of auditory sequences by temporal structure.” Canadian Journal of Psychology 43: 217–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1990. “Signs before speech.” In The Semiotic Web, Thomas A. Sebeok and Jean Umiker-Sebeok (eds), 689–755. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
. 1993. “The function of emotions in early infant communication and development.” In New Perspectives in Early Communicative Development, Jaqueline Nadel and Luigia Camaioni (eds), 48–81. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
. 1994. “Infant semiosis.” In Origins of Semiosis, Winfried Nöth (ed.), 219–252. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 1999. “Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: Evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication.” Musicae Scientiae Special Issue: Rhythm, Musical Narrative, and the Origins of Human Communication: 155–215.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, Colwyn and Penelope Hubley. 1978. “Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year.” In Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language, Andy Lock (ed.), 183–229. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tronick, Edward Z. 2003. “Things still to be done on the still-face effect.” Infancy 4 (4): 451–473. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tronick, Edward Z., Als, Heide-Lise, Adamson, Lauren, Wise, Susan and Brazelton, T. Berry. 1978. “The infant’s response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction.” American Academy of Child Psychiatry 17: 1–13. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Puyvelde, Martine, Vanfleteren, Pol, Loots, Gerrit, Deschuyffeleer, Sara, Vinck, Bart, Jacquet, Wolfgang and Verhelst, Werner. 2010. “Tonal synchrony in mother-infant interaction based on harmonic and pentatonic series.” Infant Behavior and Development 33 (4): 387–400. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wallon, Henry. 1959. “Kinesthésie et image visuelle du corps propre chez l’enfant.” Enfance 12 (3–4): 252–263. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zentner, Marcel and Eerola, Tuomas. 2010. “Rhythmic engagement with music in infancy.” PNAS 107 (13): 5768–5773. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (5)

Cited by five other publications

Galanaki, Evangelia
2023. Loneliness and intersubjectivity: A view from Trevarthen's theory. Frontiers in Psychology 14 DOI logo
Våpenstad, Eystein Victor & Brynulf Bakkenget
2021. Pre-verbal Children’s Participation in a New Key. How Intersubjectivity Can Contribute to Understanding and Implementation of Child Rights in Early Childhood. Frontiers in Psychology 12 DOI logo
Apter, Gisèle & Emmanuel Devouche
2019. Parental Psychiatric Disorders and Early Interaction: Dysregulation and Repair. In Early Interaction and Developmental Psychopathology,  pp. 107 ff. DOI logo
Apter, Gisèle, Sara Dominguez, Marie-Camille Genet & Emmanuel Devouche
2018. Accompagner la rencontre des mères souffrant d’états limites avec leur bébé. Soins Pédiatrie/Puériculture 39:300  pp. 19 ff. DOI logo
Gratier, Maya, Manuela Filippa & Gisèle Apter
2018. Bébé attentif cherche adulte(s) attentionné(s). In Bébé attentif cherche adulte(s) attentionné(s),  pp. 49 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 25 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.