Part of
Perception Metaphors
Edited by Laura J. Speed, Carolyn O'Meara, Lila San Roque and Asifa Majid
[Converging Evidence in Language and Communication Research 19] 2019
► pp. 116
References
Akpinar, E. & Berger, J.
(2015) Drivers of cultural success: The case of sensory metaphors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(1), 20–34.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ashley, R.
(2004) Musical pitch space across modalities: Spatial and other mappings through language and culture. In Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (Vol. 72). Adelaide, Australia: Causal Productions.
Barsalou, L. W.
(1999) Perceptual Perceptual symbol systems. Brain and Behavioral Science, 22(4), 577-609.Google Scholar
Berlin, B., & Kay, P.
(1964) Basic color terms: Their universality and evolution. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L.
(2000) Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75(1), 1–28.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L.
(2008) Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106, 579–593.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H.
(1973) Space, time, semantics, and the child. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp. 27–63). New York: Academic Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Croijmans, I. & Majid, A.
(2016) Not all flavor expertise is equal: The language of wine and coffee experts. PLoS ONE 5(5): e0155845.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dingemanse, M.
(2011) The meaning and use of ideophones in Siwu. Ph.D. Thesis, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen.Google Scholar
Dingemanse, M., & Majid, A.
(2012) The semantic structure of sensory vocabulary in an African language. In N. Miyake, D. Peebles, & R. P. Cooper (Eds.), Proceedings of the 34th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (CogSci 2012) (pp. 300–305). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Dolscheid, S., Shayan, S., Majid, A., & Casasanto, D.
(2013) The thickness of musical pitch: Psychophysical evidence for linguistic relativity. Psychological Science, 24(5), 613–621.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Engen, T.
(1987) Remembering odors and their names. American Scientist, 497–503.Google Scholar
Evans, N. & Wilkins, D.
(2000) In the mind’s ear: The semantic extensions of perception verbs in Australian languages. Language, 76(3), 546–592.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Geurts, K. L.
(2002) Culture and the senses: Bodily ways of knowing in an African community. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W.
(1994) The poetics of mind: Figurative thought, language and understanding. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gilead, M., Gal, O., Polak, M., & Cholow, Y.
(2015) The role of nature and nurture in conceptual metaphors: The case of gustatory priming. Social Psychology, 46, 167–173.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, R. F., Perfetti, C. A. & Schneider, W.
(2006a) Perceptual knowledge retrieval activates sensory brain regions. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(18), 4917–4921.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, R. F., Perfetti, C. A., & Schneider, W.
(2006b) Distinct and common cortical activations for multimodal semantic categories. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 6(3), 214–222.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ibarretxe-Antuñano, I.
(1999) Polysemy and metaphor in perception verbs: A cross-linguistic study. PhD Thesis. University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Idström, A. & Piirainen, E.
(2012) Endangered metaphors. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
IJzerman, H., & Semin, G. R.
(2009) The thermometer of social relations: Mapping social proximity on temperature. Psychological Science, 20(10), 1214–1220.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jamrozik, A., McQuire, M., Cardillo, E. R., Chatterjee, A.
(2016) Metaphor: Bridging embodiment to abstraction. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 23(4): 1080–1089.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, C.
(1999) Metaphor vs. conflation in the acquisition of polysemy: The case of see. In M. K. Hiraga, C. Sinha & S. Wilcox (Eds.), Cultural, psychological and typological issues in cognitive linguistics: Selected papers of the bi-annual ICLA meeting in Albuquerque, July 1995 (pp.155–170).DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kay, P., Berlin, B., Maffi, L., Merrifield, W. R., & Cook, R.
(2009) The world color survey. Stanford: Center for the Study of Language and Information.Google Scholar
Koptjevskaja-Tamm, M.
(Ed.) (2015) The linguistics of temperature. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kousta, S. T., Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Andrews, M., & Del Campo, E.
(2011) The representation of abstract words: why emotion matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(1), 14–34.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lacey, S., Stilla, R., & Sathian, K.
(2012) Metaphorically feeling: comprehending textural metaphors activates somatosensory cortex. Brain and language, 120(3), 416–421.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M.
(1980) Metaphors we live by. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Kövecses, Z.
(1987) The cognitive model of anger inherent in American English. In D. Holland & N. Quinn (Eds.), Cultural models in language and thought (pp. 195–221). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. & Turner, M.
(1989) More than cool reason: A field guide to poetic metaphor. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Le Guen, O.
(2011) Materiality vs. expressivity: The use of sensory vocabulary in Yucatec Maya. Senses & Society 6(1), pp. 117–125.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lee, S. W., & Schwarz, N.
(2012) Bidirectionality, mediation, and moderation of metaphorical effects: The embodiment of social suspicion and fishy smells. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(5), 737–749.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C.
(2000) Yélî Dnye and the theory of basic color terms. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 10(1), 3–55.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C., Majid, A., & Enfield, N. J.
(2007) Language of perception: The view from language and culture. In A. Majid (Ed.), Field Manual Volume 10 (pp. 10–21). Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C., & Majid, A.
(2014) Differential ineffability and the senses. Mind & Language, 29(4), 407–427.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lynott, D., & Connell, L.
(2009) Modality exclusivity norms for 423 object properties. Behavior Research Methods, 41(2), 558–564.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) Modality exclusivity norms for 400 nouns: The relationship between perceptual experience and surface word form. Behavior Research Methods, 45(2), 516–526.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A.
(2008) A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 102(1), 59–70.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Majid, A.
(2013) Making semantics and pragmatics “sensory”. Journal of Pragmatics, 58, 48–51.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Majid, A., & Burenhult, N.
(2014) Odors are expressible in language, as long as you speak the right language. Cognition, 130(2), 266–270.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Majid, A. & Levinson, S. C.
(2011) The senses in language and culture. Senses & Society, 6(1), 5–18.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Majid, A., Roberts, S. G., Cilissen, L., Emmorey, K., Nicodemus, B., O’Grady, L., Woll, B., LeLan, B., de Sousa, H., Cansler, B. L., Shayan, S., de Vos, C., Senft, G., Enfield, N. J., Razak, R. A., Fedden, S., Tufvesson, S., Dingemanse, M., Oztürk, O., Brown, P., Hill, C., Le Guen, O., Hirtzel, V., van Gijn, R., Sicoli, M. A., & Levinson, S. C.
in press). The differential coding of perception in the world’s languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
Matissof, J. A.
(1978) Variational semantics in Tibeto-Burman: The ‘organic’ approach to linguistic comparison. Occasional Papers of the Wolfenden Society on Tibeto-Burman Linguistics, Volume VI. Philadelphia, PA: Institute for the Study of Human Issues (ISHI).Google Scholar
Moseley, R., Carota, F., Hauk, O., Mohr, B., Pulvermüller, F.
(2012) A rol for the motor system in binding abstract emotional meaning. Cerebral Cortex, 22(7): 1634-1647.Google Scholar
Nakagawa, H.
2012The importance of taste verbs in some Khoe languages. Linguistics 50(3), 395–420.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Olofsson, J. K., & Gottfried, J. A.
(2015) The muted sense: Neurocognitive limitations of olfactory language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(6), 314–321.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Meara, C. & Majid, A.
submitted). Anger stinks in Seri: Olfactory metaphor in a lesser-described language.
Pecher, D., & Zwaan, R. A.
(Eds.) (2005) The grounding of cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thinking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
San Roque, L., Kendrick, K. H., Norcliffe, E., Brown, P., Defina, R., Dingemanse, M., Dirksmeyer, T., Enfield, N. J., Floyd, S., Hammond, J., Rossi, G., Tufvesson, S., Van Putten, S., & Majid, A.
(2015) Vision verbs dominate in conversation across cultures, but the ranking of non-visual verbs varies. Cognitive Linguistics, 26(1), 31–60.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
San Roque, L., Kendrick, K. H., Norcliffe, E. & Majid, A.
(2018) Universal meaning extensions of perception verbs are grounded in interaction. Cognitive Linguistics, 29(3), 371-406.
https://doi.org/
.Google Scholar
Schaefer, M., Denke, C., Heinze, H. J., & Rotte, M.
(2013) Rough primes and rough conversations: Evidence for a modality-specific basis to mental metaphors. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(11), 1653–1659.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmidt-Snoek, G. L., Drew, A. R., Barile, E. C., & Agauas, S. J.
(2015) Auditory and motion metaphors have different scalp distributions: an ERP study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 126.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwager, W., & Zeshan, U.
(2008) Word classes in sign languages: Criteria and classifications. Studies in Language, 32(3), 509–545.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shayan, S., Ozturk, O., & Sicoli, M. A.
(2011) The Thickness of pitch: Crossmodal metaphors in Farsi, Turkish, and Zapotec. Senses & Society, 6(1), 96–105.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Speed, L. J., & Majid, A.
(2018) An exception to mental simulation: No evidence for embodied odor language. Cognitive Science, 42(4), 1146-1178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Dutch modality exclusivity norms: Simulating perceptual modality in space. Behavior Research Methods, 49(6), 2204–2218.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stone, R. M.
(1981) Toward a Kpelle conceptualization of music performance. The Journal of American Folklore, 94(372), 188–206.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Surrallés, A.
(2016) On contrastive perception and ineffability: assessing sensory experience without colour terms in an Amazonian society. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 22(4), 962–979.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sweetser, E.
(1990) From etymology to pragmatics. New York: Cambridge University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Traugott, E. L.
(1975) Spatial expressions of tense and temporal sequencing: A contribution to the study of semantic fields. Semiotica, 15(3), 207–230.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Viberg, Å.
(1983) The verbs of perception: A typological study. Linguistics 21(1), 123–162.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vigliocco, G., Kousta, S. T., Della Rosa, P. A., Vinson, D. P., Tettamanti, M., Devlin, J. T., & Cappa, S. F.
(2013) The neural representation of abstract words: the role of emotion. Cerebral Cortex, 24(7), 1767–1777.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilkowski, B. M., Meier, B. P., Robinson, M. D., Carter, M. S., & Feltman, R.
(2009) “Hot-headed” is more than an expression: The embodied representation of anger in terms of heat. Emotion, 9(4), 464–477.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. M.
(1976) Synaesthetic adjectives: A possible law of semantic change. Language, 52(2). 461–478.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yu, N.
(1995) Metaphorical expressions of anger and happiness in English and Chinese. Metaphor and Symbol, 10(2), 59–92.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zbikowski, L.
(1998) Metaphor and music theory: Reflections from cognitive science. Music Theory Online, 4(1),1–11.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 7 other publications

BAŞ, Melike
2021. tat- Eyleminin Anlam Genişlemesi Üzerine Bir İnceleme. Akademik Dil ve Edebiyat Dergisi 5:2  pp. 1538 ff. DOI logo
EŞİM, İbrahim & Serhat ZAMAN
2022. COĞRAFYA BÖLÜMÜ LİSANS ÖĞRENCİLERİNİN “BALKANLAR” ÜZERİNE OLAN ALGILARININ METAFORLAR ARACILIĞIYLA BELİRLENMESİ. Ankara Anadolu ve Rumeli Araştırmaları Dergisi 3:5  pp. 135 ff. DOI logo
Kalda, Anu
2022. Translating perception metaphors: Linguistic, cultural and social implications. Taikomoji kalbotyra 16  pp. 86 ff. DOI logo
SPEED, LAURA J.
2021.  Bodo Winter, Sensory Linguistics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 2019. Pp. 289. ISBN: 978-9-0272-0310-6.. Language and Cognition 13:2  pp. 327 ff. DOI logo
Speed, Laura J. & Asifa Majid
2020. Grounding language in the neglected senses of touch, taste, and smell. Cognitive Neuropsychology 37:5-6  pp. 363 ff. DOI logo
Tóth, Máté
2023. A case for metonymic synesthesia. Review of Cognitive Linguistics DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 22 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.