References (99)
References
Ahlner, F., & Zlatev, J. (2010). Cross-modal iconicity: A cognitive semiotic approach to sound symbolism. Sign Systems Studies, 38(1/4), 298–348. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Akita, K. (2009). A grammar of sound-symbolic words in Japanese: Theoretical approaches to iconic and lexical properties of mimetics (unpublished doctoral dissertation). Kobe University, Japan. Retrieved from [URL]
(2015). Sound symbolism. In J.-O. Östman & J. Verschueren (Eds.), Handbook of pragmatics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergen, B. (2004). The psychological reality of phonaesthemes. Language, 80(2), 290–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berlin, B. (2006). The first congress of ethonozoological nomenclature. Journal of Royal Anthropological Institution, 12, 23–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blasi, D. E., Wichmann, S., Hammarström, H., Stadler, P. F., & Christianson, M. H. (2016). Sound-meaning association biases evidenced across thousands of languages. PNAS, 113(39), 10818–10823. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bolinger, D. L. (1946). The sign is not arbitrary. Thesaurus. Tomo V. Núms. 1, 2 y 3, 52–62.Google Scholar
Bolts, M. G., Mangigian, G. M., & Allen, M. B. (2016). Phonetic symbolism and memory for advertisements. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 30(6), 1088–1092. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, R., & Ford, M. (1961). Address in American English. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62, 375–385. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cassidy, K. W., Kelly, M. H., & Sharoni, L. J. (1999). Inferring gender from name phonology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 362–381. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., McQueen, J., & Robinson, K. (1990). Elizabeth and John: Sound patterns of men’s and women’s names. Journal of Linguistics, 26, 471–482. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Delattre, P. C., Liberman, A. M., & Cooper, F. S. (1955). Acoustic loci and transitional cues for consonants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 27, 769–773. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
D’Onofrio, A. (2014). Phonetic detail and dimensionality in sound-shape correspondences: Refining the bouba-kiki paradigm. Language and Speech, 57(3), 367–393. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Saussure, F. (1916). Course in general linguistics. London: Peter Owen.Google Scholar
Dingemanse, M., Blasi, D. E., Lupyan, G., Christianson, M. H., & Monaghan, P. (2015). Arbitrariness, iconicity and systematicity in language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(10), 603–615. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Firth, J. R. (1930). Speech. London: Benn’s Sixpenny Library.Google Scholar
Freedman, J. L., & Landauer, T. K. (1966). Retrieval of long-term memory: ‘Tip-of-the-tongue’ phenomenon. Psychonomic Science, 4, 309–310. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Funakubo, K., Kobayashi, F., Uno, R., Shinohara, K., & Odake, S. (2016). Gaisoo pakkeeji-ni okeru shokkan-hyoogen “karit”-to kisshokuji shokkan-hyoogen-no icchisei [Correspondence between texture expressions while eating food and the word karit on the package]. Paper presented at Kanto Branch, Japan Society for Bioscience, Biotechnology, and Agrochemistry, Tokyo: Nippon Veterinary and Life Science University.
Haiman, J. (1983). Iconic and economic motivation. Language, 59, 781–819. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1985a). Natural syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(Ed.) (1985b). Iconicity in syntax. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamano, S. (1998). The sound-symbolic system of Japanese. Stanford: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
(2014). Nihongo no onomatope: On’syootyoo to koozoo [Onomatopoeia in Japanese: Sound symbolism and structure]. Tokyo: Kuroshio.Google Scholar
Hawkins, J. A., & Cutler, A. (1988). Psycholinguistic factors in morphological asymmetry. In J. A. Hawkins (Ed.), Explaining language universals (pp. 280–317). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hinton, L., Nichols, J., & Ohala, J. (1994/2006). Introduction: sound-symbolic processes. In L. Hinton, J. Nichols, & J. Ohala (Eds.) Sound symbolism (pp. 1–12) Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hockett, C. (1963). The problem of universals in language. In J. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals of language (pp. 1–22). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hosokawa, Y., Atsumi, N., Uno, R., & Shinohara, K. (2018). Evil or not? Sound symbolism in Pokémon and Disney character names. Poster session presented at the 1st Conference on Pokémonastics, Keio University, Japan.
Imai, M., & Kita, S. (2014). The sound symbolism bootstrapping hypothesis for language acquisition and language evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 369, 20130298 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Irwin, F. W., & Newland, E. (1940). A genetic study of the naming of visual figures. Journal of Psychology, 9, 3–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1978). Six lectures on sound and meaning. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Janda, L. A. (2013). Quantitative methods in Cognitive Linguistics: An introduction. In L. A. Janda (ed.), Cognitive linguistics: The quantitative turn (pp. 1–32). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jespersen, O. (1922/1933). Symbolic value of the vowel i. In Phonologica. Selected papers in English, French and German (Vol. 1) (pp. 283–303). Copenhagen: Levin and Munksgaard.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and reason. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jurafsky, D. (2014). The language of food: A linguist reads the menu. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
Kawahara, S. (2017). “A” ha “i” yori ookii? Onsyootyoo de manabu onseigaku nyuumon [Introducing phonetics through sound symbolism]. Tokyo: Hituzi Shobo.Google Scholar
(2020). Sound symbolism and theoretical phonology. Language and Linguistic Compass, 14(8), e12372. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kawahara, S., Isobe, M., Kobayashi, Y., Monou, T., & Okabe, R. (2018). Acquisition of sound symbolic values of vowels and voiced obstruents by Japanese children: Using a Pokémonastics paradigm. Journal of the Phonetic Society of Japan, 22(2), 122–130.Google Scholar
Kawahara, S., Noto, A., & Kumagai, G. (2018). Sound symbolic patterns in Pokémon names. Phonetica, 75(3), 219–244. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kawahara, S., & Kumagai, G. (2019a). Expressing evolution in Pokémon names: Experimental explorations. Journal of Japanese Linguistics, 35(1), 3–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019b). Inferring Pokémon types using sound symbolism: The effects of voicing and labiality. Onsei Kenkyu [Journal of the Phonetic Society of Japan], 23(2), 111–116.Google Scholar
Kawahara, S., Godoy, M. C., & Kumagai, G. (2020). Do sibilants fly? Evidence from the sound symbolic pattern in Pokémon names. Open Linguistics, 6(1), 386–400. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kawahara, S., & Shinohara, K. (2008). A cross-linguistic study of sound symbolism: A case of voicing. Paper presented at International Conference on Language, Cognition, and Communication, Brighton University, UK.
(2011). Phonetic and psycholinguistic prominences in pun formation: Experimental evidence for positional faithfulness. In M. den Dikken & W. McClure (Eds.), Japanese/Korean Linguistics, 18 (pp. 177–188). Stanford: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
(2012). A tripartite trans-module relationship between sounds, shapes and emotions: A case of abrupt modulation. In N. Miyake, D. Peebles, & R. P., Cooper (Eds.), Proceedings of the 34th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 569–574). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Kawahara, S., Shinohara, K., & Grady, J. (2015). Iconic inferences about personality: From sounds and shapes. In M. Hiraga, W. Herlofsky, K. Shinohara, & K. Akita (Eds.), Iconicity: East meets west (pp. 57–69). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kawahara, S., Shinohara, K., & Uchimoto, Y. (2008). A positional effect in sound symbolism: An experimental study. Proceedings of the 8th Meeting of Japan Cognitive Linguistics Association, 417–427.Google Scholar
Kingston, J., & Diehl, R. (1994). Phonetic knowledge. Language, 70(3), 419–454. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Klink, R. R. (2000). Creating brand names with meaning: the use of sound symbolism. Marketing Letters, 11, 5–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Köhler, W. (1929/1947). Gestalt psychology: An introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. New York: Liveright.Google Scholar
Kumagai, G. (2019). A sound-symbolic alternation to express cuteness and the orthographic Lyman’s Law in Japanese. Journal of Japanese Linguistics, 35(1), 39–74. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020). The pluripotentiality of bilabial consonants: The images of softness and cuteness in Japanese and English. Open Linguistics, 6, 693–707. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kumagai, G., & Kawahara, S. (2017). Onsyootyoo no tyuusyoosei: Akatyan- yoo-omutu no neemingu ni okeru sin’on [How abstract is sound symbolism? Labiality in Japanese diaper names]. Proceedings of the 31st Meeting of the Phonetic Society of Japan, 49–54.Google Scholar
(2019). Pokemon no naduke ni okeru boin to yuusei-sogaion no kooka: Jikken to riron kara no apurooti [Effects of vowels and voiced obstruents on Pokémon names: Experimental and theoretical approaches]. Gengo Kenkyu [Journal of the Linguistic Society of Japan], 155, 65–99.Google Scholar
(2020). On’insosee ni motoduku onsyootyoo: Akatyan- yoo-omutu no naduke ni okeru sin’on [Feature-based sound symbolism: Labiality and diaper names in Japanese]. Gengo Kenkyu [Journal of the Linguistic Society of Japan], 157, 149–161.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal About the Mind. University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
(1999). Philosophy in the flesh. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (1987). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar: Volume I: Theoretical Prerequisites. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Levin, G., Liberman, Z., Blonk, J., & La Barbara, J. (2003). Messa di voce: an audiovisual performance and installation for voice and interactive media. Retrieved from [URL]
Lewis, M. M. (1934). Infant speech: A study of the beginnings of language. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Lindauer, S. M. (1990). The meanings of the physiognomic stimuli taketa and maluma. Bulletin of Psychonomic Society, 28(1), 47–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lockwood, G., & Dingemanse, M. (2015). Iconicity in the lab: A review of behavioral, developmental, and neuroimaging research into sound-symbolism. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–14.Google Scholar
Malkile, Y. (1990). Diachronic problems in phonosymbolism. Edita and Inedita, 1979–1980, Volume I. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Murdock, G. P. (1959). Cross-language parallels in parental kin terms. Anthropological Linguistics, 1(9), 1–5.Google Scholar
Nobile, L. (2015). Phonemes as images: An experimental inquiry into shape-sound symbolism applied to the distinctive features of French. In M. K. Higara, W. J. Herlofsky, K. Shinohara, & K. Akita (Eds.), Iconicity: East Meets West (pp. 71–91). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nooteboom, S. G. (1981). Lexical retrieval from fragments of spoken words: Beginnings vs. endings. Journal of Phonetics, 9, 407–424. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nuckolls, J. B. (1999). The case for sound symbolism. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 225–252. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ohala, J. (1984). An ethological perspective on common cross-language utilization of F0 of voice. Phonetica, 41, 1–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1994/2006). The frequency code underlies the sound symbolic use of voice pitch. In L. Hinton, J. Nichols, & J. Ohala (Eds.), Sound symbolism (pp. 325–347). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Osgood, C. E., Suci, G., & Tannenbaum, P. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana, Illinois: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Parise, C. V. & Spence, C. (2012). Audiovisual crossmodal correspondences and sound symbolism: A study using the implicit association test. Experimental Brain Research, 220(3–4), 319–333. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perniss, P., Thompson, R. L., & Vigliocco, G. (2010). Iconicity as a general property of language: Evidence from spoken and signed languages. Frontiers in Psychology. Volume 1. Article 227. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perniss, P., & Vigliocco, G. (2014). The bridge of iconicity: from a world of experience to the experiment of language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 369: 20130300. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perry, L. K., Perlman, M., Winter, B., Massaro, D. W., & Lupyan, G. (2018). Iconicity in the speech of children and adults. Developmental Science, 21(3), e12572. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peterson, R. A., & Ross, I. (1972). How to name new brand names. Journal of Advertising Research, 12(6), 29–34.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S., & Hubbard, E. M. (2001). Synesthesia: A window into perception, thought, and language. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(12), 3–34.Google Scholar
Reilly, J., Biun, D., Cowles, W., & Peelle, J. (2008). Where did words come from? A linking theory of sound symbolism and natural language evolution. Nature Proceedings. DOI logo
Sapir, E. (1929). A study in phonetic symbolism. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 225–239. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sato, Y. (2011). Shikisai-meido-to shiin-no yuuseisei-ni mirareru onshouchouteki kankei [Sound symbolic relation in brightness in color and voicing in consonants] (unpublished master’s thesis). Tokyo University of Agriculture and Technology, Japan.
Shih, S. S., Ackerman, J., Hermalin, N., Inkelas, S., & Kavitskaya, D. (2018). Pokémonikers: A study of sound symbolism and Pokémon names. Proceedings of Linguistic Society of America 2018, 3(42), 1–6. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shinohara, K., & Kawahara, S. (2009). Onshoutyou-no gengokan hikaku: yuuseisei-no imeeji-ni kansuru jikken kenkyuu [A cross-linguistic study of sound symbolism: images evoked by voicing]. Paper presented at the 26th Meeting of Japanese Cognitive Science Society. Keio University, Japan.
(2013). The sound symbolic nature of Japanese maid names. Proceedings of the 13th Annual Meeting of the Japanese Cognitive Linguistics Association, 13, 183–193.Google Scholar
(2016). A cross-linguistic study of sound symbolism: The images of size. Proceedings of the 36th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 396–410.Google Scholar
Shinohara, K., Yamauchi, N., Kawahara, S., & Tanaka, H. (2016). Takete and maluma in action: A cross-modal relationship between gestures and sounds. PLoS ONE, 11(9), e0163525. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sidhu, D. D., & Pexman, P. M. (2018). Five mechanisms of sound symbolic association. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(5), 1619–1643. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slater, A. S., & Feinman, S. (1985). Gender and the phonology of North American first names. Sex Roles, 13, 429–440. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spence, C. (2011). Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 971–995. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015). Eating with our ears: Assessing the importance of the sounds of consumption on out perception and enjoyment of multisensory flavor experiences. Flavor, 4(3). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stevens, K., & Blumstein, S. (1978). Invariant cues for place of articulation in stop consonants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 64, 1358–1368. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tessier, A.-M. (2010). Short, but not sweet: Markedness preferences and reversals in English hypocoristics. Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the Canadian Linguistic Association, Montreal, Concordia University.
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Uemura, Y. (1965). Onsei-no hyoushousei-ni tsuite [On the symbolic aspects of sounds]. Gengo seikatsu (171), 66–70. Tokyo: Chikumashobo.Google Scholar
Ultan, R. (1978). Size-sound symbolism. In J. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals of human language II: Phonology (pp. 525–568). Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Uno, R., Shinohara, K., Hosokawa, Y., Atsumi, N., Kumagai, G., & Kawahara, S. (2020). What’s in a villain’s name? Sound symbolic values of voiced obstruents and bilabial consonants. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 18(2), 428–457. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whissell, C. (2001). Cues to referent gender in randomly constructed names. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 93, 856–858. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wright, S., & Hay, J. (2002). Fred and Trema: A phonological conspiracy. In S. Benor, M. Rose, D. Sharma, J. Sweetland, & Q. Zhang (Eds.), Gendered practices in language (pp. 175–191). Stanford: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Wright, S., Hay, J., & Tessa, B. (2005). Ladies first? Phonology, frequency, and the naming conspiracy. Linguistics, 43(3), 531–561. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yorkston, E., & Menon, G. (2004). A sound idea: phonetic effects of brand names on consumer judgments. Journal of Consumer Research, 31, 43–51. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yoshida, W., & Shinohara, K. (2009). Onsee-sosee ni yoru imeeji kanki [Evoking of images by phonetic features]. Paper presented at the 26th Japanese Cognitive Science Conference, Kanagawa: Keio University, SFC.