Article published In:
Constructions and Frames
Vol. 12:2 (2020) ► pp.272314
References (65)
References
Bender, A., & Beller, S. (2014). Mapping spatial frames of reference onto time: A review of theoretical accounts and empirical findings. Cognition, 1321, 342–382. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blomberg, J. (2015). The expression of non-actual motion in Swedish, French, and Thai. Cognitive Linguistics, 261, 657–696. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017). Non-actual motion in language and experience. In I. Ibarretxe-Antuñano (Ed.), Motion and space across languages (pp. 205–227). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bohnemeyer, J. (2010). The language-specificity of conceptual structure: Path, fictive motion and time relations. In B. C. Malt & P. Wolff (Eds.), Words and the mind: How words capture human experience (pp. 111–137). Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 751, 1–28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brandt, L. (2013). The communicative mind: A linguistic exploration of conceptual integration and meaning construction. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
Caballero, R. (2009). form is motion. Dynamic predicates in English architectural discourse. In K.-U. Panther, L. L. Thornburg, & A. Barcelona (Eds.), Metonymy and metaphor in grammar (pp. 277–290). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017). Metaphorical motion constructions across specialized genres. In I. Ibarretxe-Antuñano (Ed.), Motion and space across languages (pp. 229–253). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Jasmin, K. (2012). The hands of time: Temporal gestures in English speakers. Cognitive Linguistics, 231, 643–674. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
COCA. Corpus of contemporary American English. [URL]
Clark, H. (1973). Space, time, semantics, and the child. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp. 27–63). New York: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coulson, S., & Oakley, T. (2003). Metonymy and conceptual blending. In K.-U. Panther & L. L. Thornburg (Eds.), Metonymy and pragmatic inferencing (pp. 51–80). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coulson, S., & Pagán Cánovas, C. (2009). Understanding timelines: Conceptual metaphor and conceptual integration. Cognitive Semiotics, 51, 198–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dancygier, B., & Sweetser, E. (2014). Figurative language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Duffy, S. E., & Feist, M. I. (2014). Individual differences in the interpretation of ambiguous statements about time. Cognitive Linguistics, 251, 29–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, V. (2013). Language and time: A cognitive linguistics approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Einstein, A. (1961). Relativity: The special and the general theory. [Translated by Robert W. Lawson.] New York: Three Rivers Press.Google Scholar
Fauconnier, G. (1994). Mental spaces: Aspects of meaning construction in natural language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1997). Mappings in thought and language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (2002). The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexities. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
(2008). Rethinking metaphor. In R. Gibbs (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 53–66). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fillmore, C. (1982). Frame semantics. In Linguistic Society of Korea (Ed.), Linguistics in the morning calm: selected papers from SICOL-1981 (pp. 111–137). Seoul: Hanshin.Google Scholar
Fillmore, C., & Baker, C. (2010). A frames approach to semantic analysis. In B. Heine & H. Narrog (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of linguistic analysis (pp. 313–339). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
FrameNet. [URL]
Gentner, D., Bowdle, B., Wolff, P., & Boronat, C. (2001). Metaphor is like analogy. In D. Gentner, K. Holyoak, & B. Kokinov (Eds.), The analogical mind: Perspectives from cognitive science (pp. 199–253). Cambridge (Massachusetts): The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1954). The visual perception of objective motion and subjective movement. Psychological Review, 611, 304–314. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1975). Events are perceivable but time is not. In J. T. Fraser & N. Lawrence (Eds.), The study of time II. New York: Springer-Verlag. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1986). The ecological approach to visual perception. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Grady, J. (1997a). Foundations of meaning: Primary metaphors and primary scenes. Ph.D. dissertation, University of California at Berkeley.Google Scholar
(1997b). theories are buildings revisited. Cognitive Linguistics, 81, 267–290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1999). A typology of motivation for conceptual metaphor: Correlation vs. resemblance. In R. Gibbs & G. Steen (Eds.), Metaphor in cognitive linguistics (pp. 79–100). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huumo, T. (2013). Many ways of moving along a path: What distinguishes prepositional and postpositional uses of Finnish path adpositions? Lingua, 1331, 319–355. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017). The grammar of temporal motion: A cognitive grammar account of motion metaphors of time. Cognitive Linguistics, 281, 1–43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Izutsu, K., & Izutsu, M. N. (2016). Temporal scenery: Experiential bases for deictic concepts of time in East Asian languages. In B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (Ed.), Conceptualizations of time (pp. 207–242). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1990). The invariance hypothesis: Is abstract reason based on image schemas? Cognitive Linguistics, 11, 39–74. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Turner, M. (1989). More than cool reason: A field guide to poetic metaphor. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar 11. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
(2008). Cognitive grammar: A basic introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lee, D. N. (1980). The optic flow field: The foundation of vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 290(1038), 169–179.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. (1994). Vision, shape, and linguistic description: Tzeltal body-part terminology and object description. Linguistics, 321, 791–855. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003). Space in language and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matlock, T. (2004a). The conceptual motivation of fictive motion. In G. Radden & K.-U. Panther (Eds.), Studies in linguistic motivation (pp. 221–248). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
(2004b). Fictive motion as cognitive simulation. Memory & Cognition, 321, 1389–1400. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017). Metaphor, simulation, and fictive motion. In B. Dancygier (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of cognitive linguistics (pp. 477–489). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto, Y. (1996a). Subjective motion and English and Japanese verbs. Cognitive Linguistics, 7(2), 183–226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1996b). Subjective-change expressions in Japanese and their cognitive and linguistic bases. In G. Fauconnier & E. Sweetser (Eds.), Spaces, worlds, and grammar. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McGlone, M., & Harding, J. (1998). Back (or forward?) to the future: The role of perspective in temporal language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 241, 1211–1223.Google Scholar
Moore, K. E. (2014). The spatial language of time: Metaphor, metonymy and frames of reference. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016). Elaborating time in space: The structure and function of space-motion metaphors of time. Language and Cognition, 1–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Newton, I. (1686). Philosophiae naturalis principia mathematica. Royal Society, London.Google Scholar
Özçalişkan, S., Stites, L. J., & Emerson, S. N. (2017). Crossing the road or crossing the mind: How differently do we move across physical and metaphorical spaces in speech and gesture? In I. Ibarretxe-Antuñano (Ed.), Motion and space across languages (pp. 257–277). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Radden, G., & Panther, K.-U. (Eds.). (2004). Studies in linguistic motivation. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Richardson, D., & Matlock, T. (2007). The integration of figurative language and static depictions: An eye movement study of fictive motion. Cognition, 1021, 129–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruppenhofer, J., Ellsworth, M., Petruck, M. R. L., Johnson, C. R., Baker, C. F., & Scheffczyk, J. (2016). FrameNet II: Extended theory and practice. Available on the FrameNet website ([URL]).
Stickles, E., David, O., Dodge, E., & Hong, J. (2016). Formalizing contemporary conceptual metaphor theory: A structured repository for metaphor analysis. Constructions and Frames, 8(2), 166–213. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sullivan, K. S. (2007). Grammar in metaphor: a construction grammar account of metaphoric language. Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Sweetser, E. (1997). Role and individual interpretations of change predicates. In J. Nuyts & E. Pederson (Eds.), Language and conceptualization (pp. 116–136). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Talmy, L. (2000a). Toward a cognitive semantics: Volume 1, Concept structuring systems. Cambridge (Massachusetts): MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2000b). Toward a cognitive semantics: Volume 2, Typology and process in concept structuring. Cambridge (Massachusetts): MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2017). The targeting system of language. Cambridge (Massachusetts): MIT Press.Google Scholar
(n.d.). More on fictivity. [Handout]
Zinken, J. (2010). Temporal frames of reference. In V. Evans & P. Chilton (Eds.), Language, cognition, and space: State of the art and new directions (pp. 479–498). London: Equinox.Google Scholar