Review published In:
Cognitive Linguistic Studies
Vol. 9:1 (2022) ► pp.152157
References
Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M.
(2008) Rethinking metaphor. In R. W. Gibbs, Jr. (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 53–66). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R. W.
(2017) The embodied and discourse views of metaphor: Why these are not so different and how they can be brought closer together. In B. Hampe (ed.), Metaphor: Embodied cognition and discourse (pp. 319–35). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R. W., & Cameron, L.
(2007) Social-cognitive dynamics of metaphor performance. Cognitive Systems Research, 91, 64–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kimmel, M.
(2010) Why we mix metaphors (and mix them well): Discourse coherence, conceptual metaphor, and beyond. Journal of Pragmatics, 421, 97–115. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kövecses, Z.
(2015) Where metaphors come from: Reconsidering context in metaphor. Oxford, UK: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Levels of metaphor. Cognitive Linguistics, 28 (2), 321–347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G.
(1995) Metaphor, morality, and politics, or, why conservatives have left liberals in the dust. Social Research, 62(2), 177–213.Google Scholar
Musolff, A.
(2006) Metaphor scenarios in public discourse: Metaphor and Symbol , 21(1), 23–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Steen, G.
(2017) Deliberate Metaphor Theory: Basic assumptions, main tenets, urgent issues, Intercultural Pragmatics, 14(1), 1–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar