Abdelhadi, S., Ibrahim, R., & Eviatar, Z. (2011). Perceptual load in the reading of Arabic: Effects of orthographic visual complexity on detection. Writing Systems Research, 31, 117–127.
Abd El-Minem, F.M. (1987). Elm al-sarf. Jerusalem: Al-Taufik Press [in Arabic].
Abu-Rabia, S. (2001). The role of vowels in reading semitic scripts: Data from Arabic and Hebrew. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 141, 39–59.
Adams, M.J. (1979). Models of word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 111, 133–176.
Adams, M.J., & Bruck, M. (1993). Word recognition: The interface of educational policies and scientific research. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 51, 113–139.
Aghababian, V., & Nazir, T.A. (2000). Developing normal reading skills: Aspects of the visual processes underlying word recognition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 761, 123–150.
Allen, P.A., Wallace, B., & Weber, T.A. (1995). Influence of case type, word frequency, and exposure duration on visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 211, 914–934.
Attneave, F. (1955). Symmetry, information, and memory for patterns. American Journal of Psychology, 681, 209–222.
Azzam, R. (1989). Orthography and reading of the Arabic language. In P.G. Aaron & R.M. Joshi (Eds.), Reading and writing disorders in different orthographic systems (pp. 1–29). New York: Kluwer Academic.
Azzam, R. (1993). The nature of Arabic reading and spelling errors of young children. Reading and Writing, 51, 355–385.
Babcock, M.K., & Freyd, J.J. (1988). Perception of dynamic information in static handwritten forms. American Journal of Psychology, 1011, 111–130.
Babkoff, H., Faust, M., & Lavidor, M. (1997). Lexical decision, visual hemifield and angle of orientation. Neuropsychologia, 351, 487–495.
Badian, N. (2005). Does a visual-orthographic deficit contribute to reading disability? Annals of Dyslexia, 551, 28–52.
Balota, D.A. (1994). Visual word recognition: The journey from features to meaning. In M.A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 303–358).San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Belaid, A., & Choisy, C. (2008). Human Reading based strategies for off-line arabic word recognition. Arabic and Chinese Handwriting Recognition Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 47681, 36–56.
Berent, I., & Shimron, J. (1997). The representation of Hebrew words: Evidence from the obligatory contour principle. Cognition, 641, 39–72.
Berwick D.M., & Winickoff, D.E. (1996). The truth about doctors’ handwriting: A prospective study. BMJ, 3131, 1657–1658.
Besner, D. (1989). On the role of outline shape and word-specific visual pattern in the identification of function words: None. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A1, 91–105.
Blommaert, F.J. (1988). Early-visual factors in letter confusions. Spatial Vision, 31, 199–224.
Bouma, H. (1970). Interaction effects in parafoveal letter recognition. Nature, 2261, 177–178.
Bouma, H. (1971). Visual recognition of isolated lower-case letters. Vision Research, 111, 450–474.
Bouma, H. (1973). Visual interference in the parafoveal recognition of initial and final letters of words. Vision Research, 13(4), 767–782.
Bouma, H. (1978). Visual search and reading: Eye movements and functional visual field: A tutorial review. In J. Requin (Ed.), Attention and performance, (Vol. VII1, pp. 115–146). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bouma, H., & Legein, C. (1977). Foveal and parafoveal recognition of letters and words by dyslexics and by average readers. Neuropsychologia, 151, 69–79.
Bradshaw, J.L., Nettleton, N.C., & Taylor, M.J. (1981). The use of laterally presented words in research into cerebral asymmetry: Is directional scanning likely to be a source of artifact? Brain and Language, 141, 1–14.
Briggs, R., & Hocevar, D.J. (1975). A new distinctive feature theory for upper case letters. Journal of General Psychology, 931, 87–93.
Bruder, G.A. (1978). Role of visual familiarity in the word-superiority effects obtained with the simultaneous matching task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41, 88–100.
Bryden, M.P., & Allard, F. (1976). Visual hemifield asymmetries depend on typeface. Brain & Language, 31, 191–200.
Cattell, J.M. (1896). The time taken up by cerebral operations. Mind, 111, 377–392.
Clark, J.J., & O’Regan, J.K. (1999). Word ambiguity and the optimal viewing position in reading. Vision Research, 391, 843–857.
Chung, S.T.L. (2002). The Effect of letter spacing on reading speed in central and peripheral vision. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,43(4), 1270–1276.
Chung, S.T.L., & Legge, G.E. (2009). Precision of position signals for letters. Vision Research, 491, 1948–1960.
Coltheart, M. (2005). Modeling Reading: The dual- route approach. In M. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 6–23). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Coltheart, M., & Freeman, R. (1974). Case alternation impairs word identification. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 31, 102–104.
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108(1), 204–256.
Corcoran, D.W.J., & Rouse, R.O. (1970). An aspect of perceptual organization involved in reading typed and handwritten words. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 221, 526–530.
Davis, C.J. (2010). The spatial coding model of visual word identification. Psychological Review, 1171, 713–758.
Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Sigman, M., & Vinckier, F. (2005). The neural code for written words: A proposal. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(7), 335–341.
Drewnowski, A., & Healy, A.F. (1977). Detection errors on the and and: Evidence for reading units larger than the word. Memory & Cognition, 51, 636–647.
Ehri, L. (2005). Development of sight word reading: Phases and findings. In M. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: a handbook (pp. 135–154). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
Ehri, C.L., & Snowling, J.M. (2005). Developmental variations in word recognition. In C.A. Stone, R.E. Silliman, J.B. Ehren, & K. Apel (Eds.), Handbook of language and literacy: Development and disorders (pp. 433–460). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Eviatar, Z., Ibrahim, R., & Ganayim, D. (2004). Orthography and the hemispheres: Visual and linguistic aspects of letter processing. Neuropsychology, 181, 174–184.
Eviatar, Z., & Zaidel, E. (1991). The effects of word length and emotionality on hemispheric contribution to lexical decision. Neuropsychologia, 291, 415–428.
Farah, N., Khadir, M.T., & Sellami, M. (2005). Artificial neural network fusion: Application to Arabic words recognition.
13th European Symposium on
Artificial Neural Networks ESANN
, (pp. 151–155).
Fehrer, E.V. (1935). An investigation of the learning of visually perceived forms. American Journal of Psychology, 471, 187–221.
Fisher, D.F. (1975). Reading and visual search. Memory and Cognition, 31, 188–196.
Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K.E. Patterson, J.C. Marashall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface dyslexia (pp. 301–330). London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Gervais, M.J., Harvey, L.O., Jr., & Roberts, J.O. (1984). Identification confusions among letters of the alphabet. Journal of Experimental Psychology— Human Perception and Performance, 101, 655–666.
Geyer, L.H. (1977). Recognition and confusion of the lowercase alphabet. Perception & Psychophysics, 221, 487–490.
Gibson, E.J., Osser, H., Schiff, W., & Smith, J. (1963). An analysis of critical features of letters tested by a confusion matrix. In A basic research program on reading. [Cooperative Research Project No. 639], Ithaca, NY: Cornell University
Gomez, P., Ratcliff, R., & Perea, M. (2008). The overlap model: A model of letter position coding. Psychological Review, 1151, 577– 600.
Gough, P.B. (1984).Wordrecognition. In P.D. Pearson, R. Barr, M.L. Kamil, & P. Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 225–253). New York: Longman.
Hagenzieker, M.P., van der Heijden, A.H.C., & Hagenaar, R. (1990). Time-courses in visual-information processing: Some empirical evidence for inhibition. Psychological Research, 521, 13–21.
Healy, A.F. (1976). Detection errors on the word the: Evidence for reading units larger than letters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 21, 235–242.
Healy, A.F. (1994). Letter detection:A windowto unitization and other cognitive processes in reading text. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 333–344.
Healy, A.F., Conboy, G.L., & Drewnowski, A. (1987). Characterizingthe processing units of reading: Effects of intra- and intern word spaces in a letter detection task. In B.K. Britton & S.M. Glynn) Eds.), Executive control processes in reading (pp. 279–296). Hillsdale,NJ: Erlbaum.
Hellige, J., & Adamson, M. (2007). Hemispheric Differences in processing handwritten cursive. Brain & Language, 1021, 215–227.
Henderson, J.M. (1992). Visual attention and eye movement control during reading and scene perception. In K. Rayner (Ed.), Eye movements and visual cognition: Scene perception and reading (pp. 260–283). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Iacoboni, M., & Zaidel, E. (1996). Hemispheric independence in word recognition: Evidence from unilateral and bilateral presentations. Brain and Language, 531, 121–140.
Ibrahim, R., & Eviatar, Z. (2003). The effects of metalinguistic awareness on acquisition of reading: A cross-language comparison. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Ibrahim, R., Eviatar, Z., & Aharon-Perez, J. (2002). Do the characteristics of Arabic orthography slow its cognitive processing? Neuropsychology, 161, 322–326.
Jacobs, R.J. (1979). Visual resolution and contour interaction in the fovea and periphery. Vision research, 191, 1187–1196.
Jordan, T.R., Patching, G.R., & Milner, A.D. (2000). Lateralized word recognition: Assessing the role of hemispheric specialization, modes of lexical access and perceptual asymmetry. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 261, 1192–1208.
Khateb, A., Khateb-Abdelgani, M., Taha, H.Y., & Ibrahim, R. (2014). The impact of orthographic connectivity on visual word recognition in Arabic: A cross- sectional study. Reading and Writing, 61, 1–24.
Khateb, A., Taha, H.Y., Elias, I., & Ibrahim, R. (2013). The effect of the internal orthographic connectivity of written Arabic words on the process of the visual recognition: A comparison between skilled and dyslexic readers. Writing Systems Research, 51, 214–233.
Koriat, A., & Norman, J. (1985). Reading rotated words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 490–508.
Koriat, A., & Norman, J. (1989). Why is word recognition impaired by disorientation while the identification of single letters is not? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 151, 153–163.
Kristen, P., Hensen, P., Holliday, I., & Cornelossen, P. (2006). Attentional Shifting and the role of the dorsal pathways in visual word recognition. Neuropsychologia, 441, 2926–2936.
LaBerge, D., & Brown, V. (1989). Theory of attentional operations in shape identification. Psychological Review, 961, 101–124.
LaBerge, D, & Samuels, S.J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information Processing in Reading. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 293–323.
Lachmann, T. (2000). Memory processes in recognition and comparison. Psychologia Universalis Nr. 22. Lengerich, Berlin: Pabst Science Publishers.
Latham, K., & Whitaker, D. (1996). A comparison of word recognition and reading performance in foveal and peripheral vision. Vision Research, 361, 2665–2674.
Lavidor, M. (2011). Whole-word shape effect in dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading, 34(4), 443–454.
Levi, D.M., Song, S., & Pelli, D.G. (2007). Amblyopic reading is crowded. Journal of Vision, 7(2), 211, 1–17.
Leybaert, J., & Content, A. (1995). Reading and spelling acquisition in two different teaching methods: A test of the independence hypothesis. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 71, 65–88.
Lindell, A.K., & Nicholls, M.E.R. (2003). Cortical representation of the fovea: Implications for visual half-field research. Cortex, 39(1), 111–117.
Lindell, A.K., Nicholls, M.E.R., Kwantes, P.J.K., & Castles, A. (2005). Sequential processing in hemispheric word recognition: The impact of initial letter discriminability on the OUP naming effect. Brain & Language, 931, 160–172.
Loomis, J.M. (1982). Analysis of tactile and visual confusion matrices. Perception & Psychophysics, 311, 41–52.
Lorusso, M.L., Facoetti S., Pesenti, S., Cattaneo, C., Molteni, M., & Geiger, G. (2004). Wider recognition in peripheral vision common to different subtypes of dyslexia. Vision research, 441, 2413–2424.
Mari-Beffa, P., Houghton, G., Estevez, A.F., & Fuentes, L.J. (2000). Word-based grouping affects the prime-task effect on semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 261, 469–447
Martelli, M., Majaj, J.N., & Pelli, G.D. (2005). Are faces processed like words? A diagnostic test for recognition by parts. Journal of Vision, 51, 58–70.
Marzouki, Y., Meeter, M., & Grainger, J. (2013).Location invariance in masked repetition priming of letters and words. Acta Psychologica. (Amst.), 1421, 23–29.
Mason, M. (1978). From print to sound in mature readers as a function of reader ability and two forms of orthographic regularity. Memory and Cognition, 61, 568–581.
Mazzro, D.W., & Klitzke, D. (1977). Letters are functional in word identification. Memory & Cognition,5(3), 292–298.
Mayall, K.A., Humphreys, G.W., & Olson, A. (1997). Disruption to word or letter processing? The origins of case-mixing effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 231, 1275–1286.
McClelland, J.L. (1976). Preliminary letter identification in the perception of words and nonwords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1976(7), 80–91.
McClelland, J.L., & Rumelhart, D.E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effect in letter perception. Part I: an account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 881, 375–407.
McLeish, E. (2007). A study of the effect of letter spacing on the reading speed of young readers with low vision. British Journal of Visual Impairment, 251, 133–143.
Montant, M., Nazir, T.A., & Poncet, M. (1998). Pure alexia and the viewing position effect in printed words. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 151, 93–140.
Na¨sa¨nen, R., Kukkonen, H., & Rovamo, J. (1993). Spatial integration of band-pass filtered patterns in noise. Vision Research, 331, 903–911.
Navon, D. (1978). Perception of mis-oriented words and letter strings. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 321, 129–140.
Nazir, T.A. (2000). Traces of print along the visual pathway. In A. Kennedy, R. Radach, D. Heller, & J. Pynte (Eds.), Reading as a perceptual process (pp. 3–23). Amsterdam, North- Holland: Elsevier.
Nazir, T.A., Heller, D., & Sussmann, C. (1992). Letter visibility and word recognition: The optimal viewing position in printed words. Perception & Psychophysics, 52(3), 315–328.
Nazir, T A., O'Regan, J.K., & Jaccobs, A.M. (1991). On words and their letters. Bulletin ofthe Psychonomic Society, 291, 171–174.
New, B., Ferrand, L., Pallier, C., & Brysbaert, M. (2006). Re-examining word length effects in visual word recognition: New evidence from the English Lexicon Project. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 131, 45–52.
Pammer, K., Lavis, R., Cooper, C., Hansen, P.C., & Cornelissen, P.L. (2005). Symbol- string sensitivity and adult performance in lexical decision. Brain and Language, 94(3), 278–296.
Pammer, K., Lavis, R., Hansen, P., & Cornelissen, P.L. (2004) Symbol string sensitivity and children's reading. Brain and Language, 891, 601–610.
Pelli, D.G., Burns, C.W., Farell, B., & Moore-Page, D.C. (2006) Feature detection and letter identification. Vision Research, 46(28), 4646–4674.
Pelli, D.G., & Farrell, B. (1992). Visual Memory. Talk presented at
ECVP meeting
, Pisa, Italy.
Pelli, D.G., Farell, B., & Moore, D.C. (2003) The remarkable inefficiency of word recognition. Nature, 4231, 752–756.
Pelli, D.G., Palomares, M., & Majaj, N.J. (2004). Crowding is unlike ordinary masking: Distinguishing feature integration from detection. Journal of Vision, 41, 1136–1169.
Pelli, D.G., Tillman, A.K., Freeman, J., Su, M., Berger, D.T., & Majaj, J.N. (2007). Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate. Journal of Vision, 7(2), 1–36.
Pelli, D.G., Farell, B., & Moore, D.C. (2003). The remarkable inefficiency of word recognition. Nature, 4231, 752–756.
Perea, M., & Gomez, P. (2012a). Subtle Increases in inter letter spacing facilitate the encoding of words during normal reading. PLoS ONE, 7(10), e47568.
Perea, M., & Gomez, P. (2012b). Increasing inter letter spacing facilitates encoding of words. Psychology of Bulletin & Review, 191, 332–338.
Perea, M., & Lupker, S.J. (2003). Does jugde activate COURT? Transposed-letter similarity effects in masked associative priming. Memory & Cognition, 311, 829–841.
Perea, M., & Lupker, S.J. (2004). Can CANISO activate CASINO? Transposed-letter similarity effects with nonadjacent letter positions. Journal of Memory and Language, 511, 231–246.
Perea, M., Moret-Tatay, C., & Gomez, P. (2011). The effects of inter letter spacing in visual-word recognition. Acta Psychologica, 1371, 345–351.
Perea, M., Panadero, V., Moret-Tatay, C., & Gomez, P. (2012). The effects of inter letterspacing in visual-word recognition: Evidence with young normal readers and developmental dyslexics. Learn and Instruction, 221, 420–430.
Perry, C., Ziegler, J.C., & Zorzi, M. (2007). Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: The CDP+ model of reading aloud. Psychological Review, 1141, 273–315.
Rayner, K., Foorman, B., Perfetti, C.A., Pesetsky, D., & Seidenberg, M.S. (2002). How should reading be taught? Scientific American, 2861, 84–91.
Reichle, E.D., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (2006). E–Z Reader: A cognitive-control, serial-attention model of eye-movement control during reading. Cognitive Systems Research, 71, 4–22.
Rumelhart, D.E., & McClelland, J.L. (1982). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. Psychological Review, 89(1), 60–94.
Schneider, V.I., & Healy, A.F. (1993). Detecting phonemes and letters in text: Interactions between different types and levels of processes. Memory & Cognition, 211, 739–751.
Schomaker, L., & Segers, E. (1999). Finding features used in the human reading of cursive handwriting. International Journal on Document Analysis and Recognition, 21, 13–18.
Share, D.L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an "outlier" orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 1341, 584–615.
Shillcock, R., Ellison, T.M., & Monaghan, P. (2000). Eye-fixation behavior, lexical storage, and visual word recognition in a split processing model. Psychological Review, 107(4), 824–851.
Sieroff, E., & Posner, M.I. (1988). Cueing spatial attention during processing of words and letter strings in normals. Cognitive. Neuropsycholgy, 51, 451–72.
Smith, F. (1969). Familiarity of configuration vs discriminability of features in the visual identification of words. Psychonomic Science, 141, 261–263.
Strasburger, H. (2003). A generalized cortical magnification rule predicts low-contrast letter recognition in the visual field. Journal of Vision, 3(9), 653a.
Strasburger, H. (2003). Indirektes Sehen. Formerkennung im zentralen und peripheren Gesichtsfeld. GPttingen, Bern, Toronto, Seattle: Hogrefe.
Stuart, J.A., & Burian, H.M. (1962). A study of separation difficulty and its relationship to visual acuity in normal and amblyopic eyes. American Journal of Ophthalmology, 531, 471–477.
Stuart, M., Masterson, J., Dixon, M., & Quinlan, P. (1999). Inferring sublexical correspondences from sight vocabulary: Evidence from 6- and 7-year-olds. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 52(2), 353–366.
Taha, H., Ibrahim, R., & Khateb, A. (2013). How does Arabic orthographic connectivity modulate brain activity during visual word recognition: An ERP study. Brain Topography, 261, 292–302.
Tai, Y.C., Sheedy, J., & Hayes, J. (2009). The effect of interletter spacing on reading. Paper presented at the
Computer Displays and Vision Conference
. Forest Grove, OR.
Tanaka, J.W., & Gauthier, I. (1997). Expertise in object and face recognition. In R.L. Goldstone, D.L. Medin, P.G. Schyns& (Eds.), Mechanisms of perceptual learning (Vol. 361, pp. 83–125). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Taouk, M., & Coltheart, M. (2004).The cognitive processes involved in learning to read in Arabic. Reading and Writing, 17(1-2), 27–57.
Taylor, I., & Taylor, M.M. (1983). The psychology of reading. New York: Academic Press.
Tinker, M.A. (1963). Legibility of print. Iowa: Iowa State University Press.
Townsend, J.T. (1971a). Theoretical analysis of the alphabetic confusion matrix. Perception & Psychophysics, 91, 40–50.
Townsend, J.T. (1971b). Alphabetic confusion: A test of models for individuals. Perception & Psychophysics, 91, 449–454.
Townsend, J.T., & Ashby, F.G. (1982). Experimental test of contemporary mathematical models of visual letter recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 81, 834–854.
Townsend, J.T., Hu, G.G., & Evans, R.J. (1984). Modeling feature perception in brief displays with evidence for positive interdependencies. Perception & Psychophysics, 361, 35–49.
Van Overschelde, J.P., & Healy, A.F. (2005). A blank look in reading: The effect of blank space on the identification of letters and words during reading. Experimental Psychology, 521, 213–223.
Vellutino, F.R., Fletcher, J.M., Snowling, M.J., & Scanlon, D.M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(1), 2–40.
Wagner, N.M., & Harris, L.J. (1994). Effects of typeface characteristics on visual field asymmetries for letter identification in children and adults. Brain & Language, 461, 41–58.
Watson, A.B., & Fitzhugh, A.E. (1989). Modelling character legibility. Society for Information Display Digest of Technical Papers, 201, 360–363.
Weinstein, M. (1955). Stimulus complexity and the recognition of visual patterns. Doctoral dissertation. Ohio State University
Whitney, C. (2001). How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective literature review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(2), 221–243.
Windes, J.D., (1991). Reaction time to a word in different orientations with letters rotated separately in different orientations. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 721, 299–305.
Ziegler, J.C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilledreading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychol Bulletin, 1311, 3–29.
Zorzi, M., Barbiero, C., Facoetti, A., Lonciari, L., Carrozzi, M., Montico, M., Bravar, L., George, F., Pech-Georgel, C., & Ziegler, J.C. (2012). Extra-large letter spacing improves reading in dyslexia.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
, 109(28), 11455–11459.