Review published in:
Metaphor Variation in Englishes around the World
Edited by Marcus Callies and Alexander Onysko
[Cognitive Linguistic Studies 4:1] 2017
► pp. 164169
References

References

Binder, J., & Desai, R.
(2011) The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(11), 527–536. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brandon, R., & Hornstein, N.
(1986) From icons to symbols: Some speculations on the origins of language. Biology and Philosophy, 1, 169–189.Google Scholar
[ p. 168 ]
Bunge, M.
(2014) The mind–body problem: A psychobiological approach. London: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Cardona, J., et al.
(2014) How embodied is action language? Neurological evidence from motor diseases. Cognition, 131(2), 311–322. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cevasco, J., & Marmolejo-Ramos, F.
(2013) The importance of studying the role of Prosody in the comprehension of spontaneous spoken discourse. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 45 (1), 21–33.Google Scholar
Chatterjee, A.
(2010) Disembodying cognition. Language and Cognition, 2(1), 79–116. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1994) Language and thought. Washington, D.C.: Moyer Bell.Google Scholar
Clark, A.
(1998) Embodiment and the philosophy of mind. Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplement, 43, 35–51. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dahl, C., Rasch, M., Tomonaga, M., & Adachi, I.
(2013) The face inversion effect in non-human primates revisited: An investigation in chimpanzees (Pan Troglodytes). Scientific Reports, 3. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Evans, N., & Levinson, S.
(2009) The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(05), 429–448. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fernandino, L., et al.
(2016) Concept representation reflects multimodal abstraction: A framework for embodied semantics. Cerebral Cortex, 26 (5), 2018–2034. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. & Hauser, M.
(2004) Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate. Science, 303(5656), 377–380. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, T., Fenn, K., Margoliash, D., & Nusbaum, H.
(2006) Recursive syntactic pattern learning by Songbirds. Nature, 440(7088), 1204–1207. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A.
(2015) Few believe the world is flat: How embodiment is changing the scientific understanding of cognition. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(2), 165–171. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Harnad, S.
(1990) The symbol grounding problem. Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena, 42(1–3), 335–346. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M.
(1980) The metaphorical structure of the human conceptual system. Cognitive Science, 4(2), 195–208. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lenneberg, E.
(1964) A biological perspective of language. In E. Lenneberg (Ed.), New Directions in the Study of Language (pp. 65–88). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1962) The relationship of language to the formation of concepts. Synthese, 14(2), 103–109. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Leshinskaya, A., & Caramazza, A.
(2016) For a cognitive neuroscience of concepts: moving beyond the grounding issue. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1–11. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mahon, B.
(2015) The burden of embodied cognition. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(2), 172–178. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Merritt, D., Casasanto, D., & Brannon, E.
(2010) Do monkeys think in metaphors? Representations of space and time in monkeys and humans. Cognition, 117(2), 191–202. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S.
(1995) The language instinct: The new science of language and mind. London. Penguin.Google Scholar
Poyatos, F.
(1984) The multichannel reality of discourse: Language-paralanguage-kinesics and the totality of communicative systems. Language Sciences, 6(2), 307–337. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
[ p. 169 ]
Pulvermüller, F.
(2013) How neurons make meaning: Brain mechanisms for embodied and abstract-symbolic semantics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(9), 458–470. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L., & Colunga, E.
(2012) Developing categories and concepts. In M. Spivey, K. McRae, & M. Joanisse (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 283–308). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Toivonen, I., Csúri, P., & van der Zee, E.
(Eds.) (2016) Structures in the mind. Essays on language, music, and cognition in honor of Ray Jackendoff. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Vannasing, P., et al.
(2016) Distinct hemispheric specializations for native and non-native languages in one-day-old newborns identified by fNIRS. Neuropsychologia, 84, 63–69. CrossrefGoogle Scholar