Review published In:
Metaphor Variation in Englishes around the World
Edited by Marcus Callies and Alexander Onysko
[Cognitive Linguistic Studies 4:1] 2017
► pp. 164169
References (28)
References
Binder, J., & Desai, R. (2011). The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(11), 527–536. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brandon, R., & Hornstein, N. (1986). From icons to symbols: Some speculations on the origins of language. Biology and Philosophy, 11, 169–189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bunge, M. (2014). The mind–body problem: A psychobiological approach. London: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Cardona, J., et al. (2014). How embodied is action language? Neurological evidence from motor diseases. Cognition, 131(2), 311–322. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cevasco, J., & Marmolejo-Ramos, F. (2013). The importance of studying the role of Prosody in the comprehension of spontaneous spoken discourse. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 45 (1), 21–33.Google Scholar
Chatterjee, A. (2010). Disembodying cognition. Language and Cognition, 2(1), 79–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1994). Language and thought. Washington, D.C.: Moyer Bell.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (1998). Embodiment and the philosophy of mind. Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplement, 431, 35–51. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dahl, C., Rasch, M., Tomonaga, M., & Adachi, I. (2013). The face inversion effect in non-human primates revisited: An investigation in chimpanzees (Pan Troglodytes). Scientific Reports, 31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, N., & Levinson, S. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(05), 429–448. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernandino, L., et al. (2016). Concept representation reflects multimodal abstraction: A framework for embodied semantics. Cerebral Cortex, 26 (5), 2018–2034. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. & Hauser, M. (2004). Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate. Science, 303(5656), 377–380. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentner, T., Fenn, K., Margoliash, D., & Nusbaum, H. (2006). Recursive syntactic pattern learning by Songbirds. Nature, 440(7088), 1204–1207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. (2015). Few believe the world is flat: How embodiment is changing the scientific understanding of cognition. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(2), 165–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena, 42(1–3), 335–346. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). The metaphorical structure of the human conceptual system. Cognitive Science, 4(2), 195–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lenneberg, E. (1964). A biological perspective of language. In E. Lenneberg (Ed.), New Directions in the Study of Language (pp. 65–88). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1962). The relationship of language to the formation of concepts. Synthese, 14(2), 103–109. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leshinskaya, A., & Caramazza, A. (2016). For a cognitive neuroscience of concepts: moving beyond the grounding issue. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mahon, B. (2015). The burden of embodied cognition. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(2), 172–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Merritt, D., Casasanto, D., & Brannon, E. (2010). Do monkeys think in metaphors? Representations of space and time in monkeys and humans. Cognition, 117(2), 191–202. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (1995). The language instinct: The new science of language and mind. London. Penguin.Google Scholar
Poyatos, F. (1984). The multichannel reality of discourse: Language-paralanguage-kinesics and the totality of communicative systems. Language Sciences, 6(2), 307–337. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, F. (2013). How neurons make meaning: Brain mechanisms for embodied and abstract-symbolic semantics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(9), 458–470. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, L., & Colunga, E. (2012). Developing categories and concepts. In M. Spivey, K. McRae, & M. Joanisse (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 283–308). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Toivonen, I., Csúri, P., & van der Zee, E. (Eds.) (2016). Structures in the mind. Essays on language, music, and cognition in honor of Ray Jackendoff. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Vannasing, P., et al. (2016). Distinct hemispheric specializations for native and non-native languages in one-day-old newborns identified by fNIRS. Neuropsychologia, 841, 63–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar