Review published In:
Functions of Language
Vol. 10:2 (2003) ► pp.235244
References
Aitchinson, J.
(1996) The seeds of speech: Language origin and evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Armstrong, D., W. C. Stokoe, and S. E. Wilcox
(1995) Gesture and the nature of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beaken, M.
(1996) The making of language. Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh Press.Google Scholar
Bichakjian, B. H.
(ed.) (2000) Becoming loquens: More studies in language origins. Frankfurt: Lang.Google Scholar
(2002) Language in a Darwinian perspective. Frankfurt: Lang.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D.
(1990) Language and species. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1995) Language and human behavior. Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
(1998) Catastrophic evolution: The case for a single step from protolanguage to full human language. In J. R. Hurford, M. Studdert-Kenedy, and C. Knight (eds.) Approaches to the evolution of langauge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 341–358.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1988) Language and problems of knowledge: The Managua lectures. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Deacon, T.
(1997) The symbolic species. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M.
(1996) Grooming, gossip and the evolution of language. London: Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
Givón, T. and B. F. Malle
(2002) The evolution of language out of pre-language. Amsterdam: Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hurford, J. R., M. Studdert-Kenedy, and C. Knight
(eds.) (1998) Approaches to the evolution of langauge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jenkins, L.
(2000) Biolinguistics: Exploring the Biology of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krebs, J. R. and R. Dawkins
(1984 [1978]) Animal signals: Mind reading and manipulation. In J. R. Krebs and N. B. Davies (eds.) Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach. Oxford: Blackwell. 380–402.Google Scholar
Kruskal, J., I. Dyer, and P. Black
(1971) The vocabulary method for reconstructing family trees: innovations and large scale applications. In F. R. Hodson, D. G. Kendall, and P. Tautu (eds.) Mathematics in the archaeological and historical sciences. Edinburgh: Edinburg University Press. 30–55.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P.
1998Eve spoke: Human language and human evolution. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Lock, A. and C. R. Peters
(eds.) (1996) Handbook of human symbolic evolution. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F.
(1998) The frame/content theory of evolution of speech production. Behavioral Brain Sciences 211: 499–511. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. and P. Bloom
(1990) Natural language and natural selection. Behavioural brain science 131: 707–727. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S.
(1994) The language instinct: How the mind creates language. New York: Morrow. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Velichkovsky, B. M. and D. M. Rumbaugh
(eds.) (1996) Communicating meaning: The evolution and development oflanguage. Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Wray, A.
(ed.) (2002) The transition to language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar