Review published In:
Evidentiality in language and cognition
Edited by Lena Ekberg and Carita Paradis
[Functions of Language 16:1] 2009
► pp. 149160
References (33)
References
Baicchi, Annalisa, Cristiano Broccias & Andrea Sansò (eds.). 2005. Modelling thought and constructing meaning. Cognitive models in interaction. Milan: Franco Angeli.Google Scholar
Bertele, Raphael. 2004. The typology of motion and posture verbs: a variational account. In Bernd Kortmann (ed.), Dialectology meets typology: Dialect grammar from a cross-linguistic perspective, 93–126. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Clark, Herbert H. 1996. Using language. Cambridge. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Croft, William. 2001. Radical Construction Grammar. Syntactic theory in typological perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dirven, René, Roslyn Frank & Martin Pütz (eds.). 2003. Cognitive models in language and thought. Ideology, metaphors and meaning. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Enfield, Nick & Anna Wierzbicka. 2002. The body in description of emotion. Pragmatics and Cognition 101: 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fauconnier, Giles & Mark Turner. 1996. Blending as a central process of grammar. In Adele Goldberg (ed.) Conceptual structure, discourse and language, 113–130. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Geeraerts, Dirk. 2003. Cultural models of linguistic standardization. In René Dirven, Roslyn Frank & Martin Pütz (eds.). 2003. Cognitive models in language and thought. Ideology, metaphors and meaning, 25–68. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Geeraerts, Dirk, Stefan Grondelaers & Peter Bakema. 1994. The structure of lexical variation. Meaning, naming, and context. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, Adele. 1995. Constructions. A Construction Grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Gonzálvez-García, Francisco & Christopher S. Butler. 2006. Mapping functional-cognitive space. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics 41: 39–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harder Peter. 2003. The status of linguistic facts. Rethinking the relation between cognition, social institution and utterance from a functional point of view. Mind and Language 181: 52–76. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Holland, Dorothy & Naomi Quinn (eds.). 1987. Cultural models in language and thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Horn, Lawrence. 1984. Toward a new taxonomy for pragmatic inference: Q- and R-based implicature. In Deborah Schiffrin (ed.). Meaning, from, and use in context, 11–42. Washington DC.: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Itkonen, Esa. 2003. What is Language? A study in the philosophy of linguistics. Turku: Abo Akadeemis Tryckeri.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray. 2000. Foundations of language: brain, meaning, grammar, evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kristiansen, Gitte. 2003. How to do things with allophones: linguistic stereotypes as cognitive reference points in social cognition. In René Dirven, Roslyn Frank & Martin Pütz (eds.), Cognitive models in language and thought. Ideology, metaphors and meaning, 69–120. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Langacker, Ronald. 1987. Foundations of Cognitive Grammar. Volume I. Theoretical Prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
. 1991. Foundations of Cognitive Grammar. Volume II. Descriptive Applications. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
. 1998. Conceptualization, symbolization, and grammar. In Michael Tomasello (ed.), The new psychology of language: Cognitive and functional approaches to language structure, 1–41. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
. 2001. Discourse in Cognitive Grammar. Cognitive Linguistics 12(2). 143–188. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levelt, Willelm J. M. 1989. Speaking. From intention to articulation. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Levinson, Stephen. 2000. Presumptive Meaning. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Panther, Klaus Uwe & Linda Thornburg. 1998. A cognitive approach to inferencing in conversation. Journal of Pragmatics 301. 755–769. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2003. Metonymies as natural inference and activation schemas: the ease of dependent clauses as independent speech acts. In Klaus Uwe Panther & Linda Thornburg (eds.), Metonymy and pragmatic inferencing, 127–147. Amsterdam: Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza, Francisco José. 1996. Understanding through metonymy: The role of metonymy in communication and cognition. In Beatrice Peñas (ed.), The pragmatics of understanding and misunderstanding, 197–208. Zaragoza: University of Zaragoza.Google Scholar
. 1998. On the nature of blending as a cognitive phenomenon. Journal of Pragmatics 30(3). 259–274. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza, Francisco José & Olga Diez. 2002. Patterns of conceptual integration. In René Dirven & Ralph Pörings (eds.), Metaphor and metonymy in comparison and contrast, 489–352. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sinha, Chris. 1999. Situated selves. In Joan Bliss, Roger Saljo & Paul Light (eds.), Learning sites: Social and technological resources for learning, 32–46. Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Speelman, Dirk, Stefan Grondalaers & Dirk Geeraerts. 2003. Profile-based linguistic uniformity as a generic method for comparing language varieties. Computers and the Humanities 371. 317–337. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperber, Dan & Deidre Wilson. 1995. Relevance: Communication and cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Talmy, Leonard. 2000. Towards cognitive semantics. Cambridge, Mass. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Zlatev, Jordan. 2001. The epigenesis of meaning in human beings, and possibly in robots. Mind and Machines 111. 155–195. DOI logoGoogle Scholar