Article published in:
On mood and speech function and the ‘why’ of text analysis: In honour of Margaret Berry
Edited by Lise Fontaine, Miriam Taverniers and Kristin Davidse
[Functions of Language 26:1] 2019
► pp. 112135
References

References

Austin, John L.
1962How to do things with words. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan, Revere Perkins & William Pagliuca
1994The evolution of grammar: Tense, aspect, and modality in the languages of the world. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Byrne, Richard W.
2006Parsing behavior: a mundane origin for an extraordinary ability. In Nicholas J. Enfield & Stephen C. Levinson (eds.), Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and interaction, 478–505. Oxford: Berg.Google Scholar
Clark, Herbert H.
2016Depicting as a method of communication. Psychological Review 123 (3). 324–347. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Herbert H. & Richard J. Gerrig
1990Quotations as demonstrations. Language 66. 764–805. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Comrie, Bernard
2003On explaining language universals. In Michael Tomasello (ed.), The new psychology of language: Cognitive and functional approaches to language structure. Volume 2, 195–209. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Corballis, Michael C.
1983Human laterality. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Darwin, Charles
1898The expression of the emotions in man and animals. New York, NY: D. Appleton and Company.Google Scholar
Davidse, Kristin, Lieven Vandelanotte & Hubert Cuyckens
(eds.) 2010Subjectification, intersubjectification and grammaticalization. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, Iain & W. Noble
1993Tools and language in human evolution. In Kathleen R. Gibson & Tim Ingold (eds.), Tools, language and cognition in human evolution, 363–388. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Deacon, Terrence
1997The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the human brain. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
DeLoache, Judy S.
2004Becoming symbol-minded. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8 (2). 66–70. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dugatkin, Lee Alan & Lyudmila Trut
2017How to tame a fox (and build a dog): Visionary scientists and a Siberian tale of jump-started evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, Robin
1996Grooming, gossip and the evolution of language. London: Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
2010How many friends does one person need? Dunbar’s number and other evolutionary quirks. London: Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
[ p. 133 ]
2012Gossip and the social origins of language. In Maggie Tallerman & Kathleen R. Gibson (eds.), 343–345.Google Scholar
Fitch, W. Tecumseh
2010The evolution of language. Cambridge: CUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Frith, Chris D. & Uta Frith
2007Social cognition in humans. Current Biology 17. R724–R732. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ghomeshi, Jila, Ray Jackendoff, Nicole Rosen & Kevin Russell
2004Contrastive focus reduplication in English (the salad-salad paper). Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 22. 307–357. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, Kathleen R.
2012Tool-dependent foraging strategies and the origin of language. In Maggie Tallerman & Kathleen R. Gibson (eds.), 340–342.Google Scholar
Givón, Talmy
1995Functionalism and grammar. Amsterdam: Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goodall, Jane
1986The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behaviour. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gosden, Chris
2003Prehistory: A very short introduction. Oxford: OUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K.
1970Language structure and language function. In John Lyons (ed.), New horizons in linguistics, 140–165. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
1973Explorations in the functions of language. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
1975Learning how to mean: Explorations in the development of language and meaning. London: Edward Arnold. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
1979Modes of meaning and modes of expression: Types of grammatical structure and their determination by different semantic functions. In D. J. Allerton, Edward Carney & David Holdcroft (eds.), Function and context in linguistic analysis: Essays offered to William Haas, 57–79. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
1985An introduction to functional grammar. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
2004On grammar as the driving force from primary to higher-order consciousness. In Geoff Williams & Annabelle Lukin (eds.), 15–44.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. & Christian M. I. M. Matthiessen
2014Halliday’s introduction to functional grammar. London: Routledge. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hare, Brian, Michelle Brown, Christina Williamson & Michael Tomasello
2002The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science 298: 1634–1636. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hare, Brian & Michael Tomasello
2005Human-like social skills in dogs. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(9). 439–444. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hasan, Ruqaiya
2015Systemic Functional Linguistics: Halliday and the evolution of a social semiotic. In Jonathan J. Webster (ed.), The Bloomsbury companion to M. A. K. Halliday, 101–134. London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Haslam, Michael
2019The other tool users. Scientific American 320 (3). 58–63.Google Scholar
Hengeveld, Kees & J. Lachlan Mackenzie
2008Functional Discourse Grammar: A typologically-based theory of language structure. Oxford: OUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hernandez-Aguilar, R. Adriana, Jim Moore & Travis R. Pickering
2007Savanna chimpanzees use tools to harvest the underground storage organs of plants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 104. 19210–19213. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
[ p. 134 ]
Hobson, Peter
2004The cradle of thought: Exploring the origins of thinking. London: Pan Books.Google Scholar
Hockett, Charles F.
1960The origin of speech. Scientific American 203(3). 88–96. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, Gavin R. & Russell D. Gray
2003Diversification and cumulative evolution in New Caledonian crow tool manufacture. Proceedings of the Royal Society London, B 270. 867–874. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kimura, Doreen
1993Neuromotor mechanisms in human communication. Oxford: OUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, Stephen C.
2006On the human “interaction engine”. In Nicholas J. Enfield & Stephen C. Levinson (eds.), Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and interaction, 39–69. Oxford: Berg.Google Scholar
Matthiessen, Christian M. I. M.
2004The evolution of language: A systemic functional exploration of phylogenetic phases. In Geoff Williams & Annabelle Lukin (eds.), 45–90.Google Scholar
McGregor, William B.
1997Semiotic grammar. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
2013aOptionality in grammar and language use. Linguistics 51(6). 1147–1204. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
2013bThere are existential constructions and existential constructions: Presumption invoking existentials in English. Folia Linguistica 47(1). 139–181. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
2017There’s grammar and there’s grammar just as there’s usage and there’s usage. English Text Construction 10(2). 199–232. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, T. J. H., N. T. Uomini, L. E. Rendell, L. Chouinard-Thuly, S. E. Street, H. M. Lewis, C. P. Cross, C. Evans, R. Kearney, I. de la Torre, A. Whiten and K. N. Laland
2015Experimental evidence for the co-evolution of hominin tool-making teaching and language. Nature Communications 6. 6029. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Painter, Claire
1984Into the mother tongue: A case study in early language development. London: Pinter.Google Scholar
1991Learning the mother tongue. Geelong: Deakin University Press.Google Scholar
2004The ‘interpersonal first’ principle in child language development. In Geoff Williams & Annabelle Lukin (eds.), 133–153.Google Scholar
Rose, David
2006A Systemic Functional approach to language evolution. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 16(1). 73–96. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, Robert M. & Dorothy L. Cheney
2012Primate social cognition as a precursor to language. In Maggie Tallerman & Kathleen R. Gibson (eds.), 59–70.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, Robert M., Dorothy L. Cheney & Thore J. Bergman
2005Primate social cognition and the origins of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(6). 264–266. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tallerman, Maggie & Kathleen R. Gibson
(eds.) 2012The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Tannen, Deborah
1987Repetition in conversation: Toward a poetics of talk. Language 63. 574–605. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
1989Talking voices: Repetition, dialogue and imagery in conversational discourse. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Terrace, Herbert S., L. A. Petitto, R. J. Sanders & T. G. Bever
1979Can an ape create a sentence? Science 206(4421). 891–902. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
[ p. 135 ]
Tomasello, Michael
1999The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
2003aConstructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
2003bThe key is social cognition. In Dedre Gentner & Susan Goldin-Meadow (eds.), Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought, 47–57. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
2008Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
2014A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Torr, Jane
2015Language development in early childhood: Learning how to mean. In Jonathan J. Webster (ed.), The Bloomsbury companion to M. A. K. Halliday, 242–256. London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth Closs
2006Historical aspects of modality. In William Frawley (ed.), The expression of modality, 107–139. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
2011Modality from a historical perspective. Language and Linguistics Compass 5(6). 381–396. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Williams, Geoff & Annabelle Lukin
(eds.) 2004The development of language: Functional perspectives on species and individuals. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Wynn, Thomas
2012The paleolithic record. In Maggie Tallerman & Kathleen R. Gibson (eds.), 282–295.Google Scholar