Chapter published in:
Figurative Language – Intersubjectivity and Usage
Edited by Augusto Soares da Silva
[Figurative Thought and Language 11] 2021
► pp. 1940
References

References

Arslan, B., Taatgen, N. A., & Verbrugge, R.
(2017) Five-year-olds’ systematic errors in second-order false belief tasks are due to first-order theory of mind strategy selection: A computational modeling study. Frontiers in Psychology 8 , 275. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Barnden, J.
(2017) Irony, pretence and fictively-elaborating hyperbole. In A. Athanasiadou, & H. L. Colston (Eds.), Irony in language use and communication (pp. 145–178). Amsterdam: Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U.
(1985) Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition, 21 , 37–46. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bohn, M., & Köymen, B.
(2018) Common ground and development. Child Development Perspectives, 12 , 104–108. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brems, L., Ghesquière, L., & Van de Velde, F.
(2012) Intersections of intersubjectivity. English Text Construction, 5 , 1–6. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H.
(1996) Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Corballis, M.
(2014) The recursive mind: The origins of human language, thought, and civilization. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Dancygier, B., Lu, W., & Verhagen, A.
(Eds.) (2016) Viewpoint and the fabric of meaning: Form and use of viewpoint tools across languages and modalities. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
De Jaegher, H., & Di Paolo, E.
(2007) Participatory sense-making: An enactive approach to social cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6 , 485–507. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
DeLancey, S.
(1981) An interpretation of split ergativity and related patterns. Language, 57 , 626–57. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Di Meola, C.
(1994) Kommen und gehen: Eine kognitiv-linguistische Untersuchung der Polysemie deiktischer Bewegungsverben. Tübingen: Niemeyer.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R.
(2000) On the origin of the human mind. In P. Carruthers (Ed.), Evolution and the human mind: Modularity, language and meta-cognition (pp. 238–253). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Foucault, M.
(1966) Les mots et les choses. Paris: Gallimard.Google Scholar
Fusaroli, R., Demuru, P., & Borghi, A. M.
(Eds.) (2012) The intersubjectivity of embodiment. Thematic issue of Journal of Cognitive Semiotics, 4 , 1–250.Google Scholar
Gallagher, S.
(2012) Phenomenology. Palgrave MacMillan.Google Scholar
Geeraerts, D.
(1985) Paradigm and paradox: Explorations into a paradigmatic theory of meaning and its epistemological background. Leuven: Universitaire Pers.Google Scholar
(1993) Vagueness’s puzzles, polysemy’s vagaries. Cognitive Linguistics, 4 , 223–272. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Geeraerts, D., & Grondelaers, S.
(1995) Looking back at anger. Cultural traditions and metaphorical patterns. In J. Taylor, & R. E. MacLaury (Eds.), Language and the construal of the world (pp. 153–180). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Geeraerts, D.
(2016) The sociosemiotic commitment. Cognitive Linguistics, 27 , 527–542. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grueneisen, S., Wyman, E., & Tomasello, M.
(2015) “I know you don’t know I know…” Children use second-order false-belief reasoning for peer coordination. Child Development, 86 , 287–293. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Harder, P.
(2010) Meaning in mind and society: A functional contribution to the social turn in Cognitive Linguistics. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaiser, E., Runner, J. T., Sussman, R. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K.
(2009) Structural and semantic constraints on the resolution of pronouns and reflexives. Cognition, 112 , 55–80. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kempson, R.
(1977) Semantic theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kinderman, P., Dunbar, R., & Bentall, R. P.
(1998) Theory-of‐mind deficits and causal attributions. British Journal of Psychology, 89 , 191–204. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Krebs, J. R., & Dawkins, R.
(1984) Animal signals: Mind reading and manipulation. In J. R. Krebs, & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach (pp. 380–402). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kristiansen, G., & Dirven, R.
(Eds.) (2006) Cognitive sociolinguistics: Language variation, cultural models, social systems. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Levinas, E.
(1961) Totalité et infini : Essai sur l’extériorité. Paris: Le Livre de Poche.Google Scholar
Levinson, S.
(2003) Space in language and cognition: Explorations in cognitive diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meijering, B., van Rijn, H., Taatgen, N. A., & Verbrugge, R.
(2011) I do know what you think I think: Second-order theory of mind in strategic games is not that difficult. Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 2486–2491.Google Scholar
Miller, S. A.
(2012) Theory of mind: Beyond the preschool years. New York: Psychology Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Morton, J.
(1986) Developmental contingency modelling. A framework for discussing the processes of chang and the consequence of deficiency. Advances in Psychology, 36 , 141–165. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
O’Grady, C., Kliesch, C., Smith, K., & Scott-Phillips, T. C.
(2015) The ease and extent of recursive mindreading, across implicit and explicit tasks. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36 , 313–322. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G.
(1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4 , 515–526. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Preston, S. D., & De Waal, F. B. M.
(2002) Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25 , 1–20. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. W.
(1978) When is attribution of beliefs justified? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4 , 592–593. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ruthrof, H.
(2015) Implicit deixis. Language Sciences, 47 , 107–116. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Saunders, G.
(2013) Tenth of December. London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R.
(1976) A classification of illocutionary acts. Language in Society, 5 , 1–23. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Silverstein, M.
(1976) Hierarchy of features and ergativity. In Robert M. W. Dixon (Ed.), Grammatical categories in Australian languages (pp. 112–171). Atlantic Highlands, NJ: Humanities Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, D.
(2000) Metarepresentations in an evolutionary perspective. In D. Sperber (Ed.), Metarepresentations: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 117–137). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tobin, V., & Israel, M.
(2012) Irony as a viewpoint phenomenon. In B. Dancygier, & E. Sweetser (Eds.), Viewpoint in language: A multimodal perspective (pp. 25–46). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2008) Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014) A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Valle, A., Massaro, D., Castelli, I., & Marchettia, A.
(2015) Theory of mind development in adolescence and early adulthood: The growing complexity of recursive thinking ability. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 11 , 112–124. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Verhagen, A.
(2005) Constructions of intersubjectivity: Discourse, syntax, and cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2007) Construal and perspectivization. In D. Geeraerts, & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 48–81). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2015) Grammar and cooperative communication. In E. Dąbrowska, & D. Divjak (Eds.), Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 232–252). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J.
(2001) Meta-analysis of theory of mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72 , 655–684. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J.
(1983) Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13 , 103–128. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ziemke, T., Zlatev, J., & Frank, R.
(Eds.) (2007) Body, language and mind 1: Embodiment. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Zlatev, J.
(2008) The co-evolution of intersubjectivity and bodily mimesis. In J. Zlatev, T. Racine, C. Sinha, & E. Itkonen (Eds.), The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity (pp. 215–244). Amsterdam: Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zlatev, J., Racine, T., Sinha, C., & Itkonen, E.
(Eds.) (2008) The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity. Amsterdam: Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar