Article published In:
GestureVol. 15:3 (2016) ► pp.321–339
The temporal relationship between speech and manual communicative gesture in children with specific language
impairment
This study examined the relationship between word frequency and timing of communicative gestures in children with specific language
impairment (SLI) and typically-developing (TD) children. Nine children with SLI and twelve age-matched TD children produced a narrative
after watching an animated cartoon. Redundant gesture-speech pairs were identified and coded for temporal alignment between gesture and
speech onset and gesture duration. Word frequency for the co-occurring words was determined using the SUBTLEXus database. No significant
group differences were found for temporal alignment or gesture duration. However, word frequency was associated with temporal alignment and
gesture duration in TD children, but not in children with SLI. This finding suggests that the role communicative gestures play in lexical
access may be different in children with SLI relative to TD children.
References (34)
Alibali, Martha W., Sotaro Kita, & Amanda J. Young (2000). Gesture and the process of speech production: We think, therefore we gesture. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15 (6), 593–613.
Alt, Mary & Elena Plante (2006). Factors that influence lexical and semantic fast mapping of young children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 491, 941–954.
Blake, Joanna, Debbie Myszczyszyn, Ariela Jokel, & Neda Bebiroglu (2008). Gestures accompanying speech in specifically language-impaired children and their timing with speech. First Language, 281, 237–253.
Brown, Linda, Rita J. Sherbenou, & Susan K. Johnsen (1990). Test of nonverbal intelligence – 2. Austin, TX: ProEd.
Brysbaert, Marc & Boris New (2009). SubtlexUS. [URL] (accessed 15 October 2013).
Burgemeister, Bessie B., Lucille Hollander Blum, & Irving Lorge (1972). Columbia mental maturity scales. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich Inc.
de Marchena, Ashley & Inge-Marie Eigsti (2010). Conversational gestures in autism spectrum disorders: Asynchrony but not decreased frequency. Autism Research, 31, 311–322.
Evans, Julia L., Martha W. Alibali, & Nicole M. McNeil (2001). Divergence of verbal expression and embodied knowledge: Evidence from speech and gesture in children with specific language impairment. Language and Cognitive Processes, 161, 309–331.
Hallgren, Kevin A. (2012). Computing inter-rater reliability for observational data: An overview and tutorial. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 8 (1), 23–34.
Hill, Elisabeth L. (1998). A dyspraxic deficit in specific language impairment and developmental coordination disorder? Evidence from hand and arm movements. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 401, 388–395.
Hill, Elisabeth L. (2001). Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: A review of the literature with regards to concomitant motor impairments. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36 (2), 149–171.
Hill, Elisabeth L., Dorothy V.M. Bishop, & Ian Nimmo-Smith (1998). Representational gestures in developmental co-ordination disorder and specific language impairment: Error-types and the reliability of ratings. Human Movement Science, 171, 655–678.
Hsu, Hsinjen J. & Dorothy V.M. Bishop (2014). Sequence-specific procedural learning deficits in children with specific language impairment. Developmental Science, 17 (3), 352–365.
Iverson, Jana M. & Barbara A. Braddock (2011). Gesture and motor skill in relation to language in children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 541, 72–86.
Kan, Pui F. & Jennifer Windsor (2010). Word learning in children with primary language impairment: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 531, 739–756.
Lahey, Margaret & Jan Edwards (1999). Naming errors of children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 421, 195–205.
Leonard, Laurence B. (2014). Children with specific language impairment(2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Mainela-Arnold, Elina, Julia L. Evans, & Martha W. Alibali (2006). Understanding conservation delays in children with speicifc language impairment: Task representations revealed in speech and gesture. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 491, 1267–1279.
Mainela-Arnold, Elina, Julia L. Evans, & Jeffry A. Coady (2008). Lexical representations in children with SLI: Evidence from a frequency-manipulated gating task. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 511, 381–393.
Mainela-Arnold, Elina, Julia L. Evans, & Jeffry A. Coady (2010). Explaining lexical semantic deficits in specific language impairment: The role of phonological similarity, phonological working memory, and lexical competition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 531, 1742–1756.
Mainela-Arnold, Elina, Martha W. Alibali, Kristin Ryan, & Julia L. Evans (2011). Knowledge of mathematical equivalence in children with specific language impairment: Insights from gesture and speech. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 421, 18–30.
Mainela-Arnold, Elina, Martha W. Alibali, Autumn B. Hostetter, & Julia L. Evans (2014). Gesture-speech integration in children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 49 (6), 761–770.
McGregor, Karla K., Robyn M. Newman, Renée M. Reilly, & Nina C. Capone (2002). Semantic representation and naming in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 451, 998–1014.
McNeill, David (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Morrel-Samuels, Palmer & Robert M. Krauss (1992). Word familiarity predicts temporal asynchrony of hand gesture and speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18 (3), 615–622.
Rauscher, Frances H., Robert M. Krauss, & Yihsiu Chen (1996). Gesture, speech, and lexical access: The role of lexical movements in speech production. Psychological Science, 7 (4), 226–231.
Rice, Mabel L. & Kenneth Wexler (1996). Toward tense as a clinical marker of specific language impairment in English-speaking children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 391, 1239–1257.
Roid, Gale H. & Lucy J. Miller (1997). Leiter international performance scale, revised. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting.
Semel, Eleanor, Elisabeth H. Wiig, & Wayne A. Secord (1987). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals – revised (CELF-R.). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.
Sheng, Li & Karla K. McGregor (2010). Lexical-semantic organization in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 531, 146–159.
Ullman, Michael T. & Myrna Gopnik (1999). Inflectional morphology in a family with inherited specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 201, 61–117.
Vukovic, Mile, Irena Vukovic, & Vesna Stojanovik (2010). Investigation of language and motor skills in Serbian speaking children with specific language impairment and in typically developing children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 311, 1633–1644.
Zelaznik, Howard N. & Lisa Goffman (2010). Generalized motor abilities and timing behavior in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53 (4), 383–393.
Cited by (2)
Cited by two other publications
Bellifemine, Corrado & Loulou Kosmala
2024.
Investigating pausing and gesturing patterns in children with and without developmental language disorder.
Multimodal Communication
Clarke, Jean S., Nicholas Llewellyn, Joep Cornelissen & Rowena Viney
2021.
Gesture Analysis and Organizational Research: The Development and Application of a Protocol for Naturalistic Settings.
Organizational Research Methods 24:1
► pp. 140 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 7 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.