Article published In:
Anthropology of Gesture
Edited by Heather Brookes and Olivier Le Guen
[Gesture 18:2/3] 2019
► pp. 209238
References (111)
References
Archer, D. (1997). Unspoken diversity: Cultural differences in gestures. Qualitative Sociology, 20 (1), 79–105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, J. B., Chovil, N., Lawrie, D. A., & Wade, A. (1992). Interactive gestures. Discourse Processes, 151, 469–489. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beaupoil-Hourdel, P., Morgenstern, A., & Boutet, D. (2016). A child’s multimodal negations from 1 to 4: The interplay of modalities. In P. Larrivée & C. Lee (Eds.), Negation and polarity: Experimental perspectives (pp. 95–123). Cham: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Best, E. (1924). The Maori, Vol. 11. Wellington, NZ: H. H. Tombs.Google Scholar
Bonvillian, J. D., Ingram, V. L., & McCleary, B. M. (2009). Observations on the use of manual signs and gestures in the communicative interactions between Native Americans and Spanish explorers of North America: The accounts of Bernal Díaz del Castillo and Álvar Núñez Cabeza de Vaca. Sign Language Studies, 9 (2), 132–165. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brentari, D., Marotta, G., Margherita, I., & Ott, A. (2013). The interaction of pitch accent and gesture production in Italian and English. Studi e saggi linguistici, 51 (1), 79–97.Google Scholar
Bressem, J. & Müller, C. (2014). The family of Away gestures: Negation, refusal, and negative assessment. In C. Müller, A. Cienki, E. Fricke, S. H. Ladewig, D. McNeill, & J. Bressem (Eds.), Body – language – communication (Vol. 21, pp. 1592–1604). Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Bressem, J., Stein, N., & Wegener, C. (2017). Multimodal language use in Savosavo: Refusing, excluding and negating with speech and gesture. Pragmatics, 27 (2), 173–206. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brookes, H. (2004). A repertoire of South African quotable gestures. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 14 (2), 186–224. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bulwer, J.. (1975 [orig. 1644]). Chirologia: Or the natural language of the hand and Chironomia: Or the art of manual rhetoric. J. W. Cleary (Ed.). Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Calbris, G. (1990). The semiotics of French gestures. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Carayon, C. (2016). “The gesture speech of mankind”: Old and new entanglements in the histories of American Indian and European sign languages. American Historical Review, 121 (2), 461–491. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Jasmin, K. (2012). The hands of time: Temporal gesture in English speakers. Cognitive Linguistics, 23 (4), 643–674. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003). Pointing and placing. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 243–268). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
(2016). Depicting as a method of communication. Psychological Review, 123 (3), 324–347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cooperrider, K. (2017). Foreground gesture, background gesture. Gesture, 16 (3), 176–202. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018, May 14). From pointing to nodding: Is gesture a universal language? Aeon. Retrieved from: [URL]
Cooperrider, K. & Núñez, R. (2012). Nose-pointing: Notes on a facial gesture of Papua New Guinea. Gesture, 12 (2), 103–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cooperrider, K., Abner, N., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2018). The palm-up puzzle: Meanings and origins of a widespread form in gesture and sign. Frontiers in Communication, 3 (June), 1–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cooperrider, K., Slotta, J., & Núñez, R. (2018). The preference for pointing with the hand is not universal. Cognitive Science, 42 (4), 1375–1390. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cooperrider, K., Núñez, R., & Sweetser, E. (2014). The conceptualization of time in gesture. In C. Müller, A. Cienki, E. Fricke, S. Ladewig, D. McNeill, & J. Bressem (Eds.), Body – language – communication (Vol. 21, pp. 1781–1788). New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of emotions in man and the animals. London: John Murray. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Ruiter, J. P. & Wilkins, D. P. (1998). The synchronization of gesture and speech in Dutch and Arrernte (an Australian Aboriginal language): A cross-cultural comparison. In S. Santi (Ed.), Oralité et gestualité (pp. 603–607). Paris: Harmattan.Google Scholar
Defina, R. (2016). Do serial verb constructions describe single events? A study of co-speech gestures in Avatime. Language, 92 (4), 890–910. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dégerando, J.-M. (1969 [orig. 1800]). The observation of savage peoples (trans. F. C. T. Moore). London: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dingemanse, M. (2013). Ideophones and gesture in everyday speech. Gesture, 13 (2), 143–165. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dingemanse, M. & Floyd, S. (2014). Conversation across cultures. In N. J. Enfield, P. Kockelman, & J. Sidnell (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of linguistic anthropology (pp. 447–480). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dingemanse, M., Roberts, S. G., Baranova, J., Blythe, J., Drew, P., Floyd, S., [&], & Enfield, N. J. (2015). Universal principles in the repair of communication problems. Plos One, 10 (9), e0136100. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eco, U. (1976). A theory of semiotics. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Efron, D. (1941). Gesture and environment. New York: Kings Crown Press.Google Scholar
(2009). The anatomy of meaning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Enfield, N. J., Kita, S., & de Ruiter, J. P. (2007). Primary and secondary pragmatic functions of pointing gestures. Journal of Pragmatics, 39 (10), 1722–1741. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Engelke, M. (2018). How to think like an anthropologist. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Evans, N. (2010). Dying words: Endangered languages and what they have to tell us. London: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Evans, N., & Levinson, S. C. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32 (5), 429–448. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Everett, D. L. (2005). Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã: Another look at the design features of human language. Current Anthropology, 46 (4), 621–646. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fox Tree, E. (2009). Meemul Tziij: An indigenous sign language complex of Mesoamerica. Sign Language Studies, 9 (3), 324–366. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Floyd, S. (2016). Modally hybrid grammar? Celestial pointing for time-of-day reference in Nheengatú. Language, 92 (1), 31–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Floyd, S., Manrique, E., Rossi, G., & Torreira, F. (2016). Timing of visual bodily behavior in repair sequences: Evidence from three languages. Discourse Processes, 53 (3), 175–204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gawne, L. & McCulloch, G. (2019). Emoji as digital gestures. Language@Internet, 171, article 2.Google Scholar
Goldenweiser, A. A. (1913). The principle of limited possibilities in the development of culture. The Journal of American Folklore, 26 (101), 259–290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, J. (1978). Generalizations about numeral systems. In J. Greenberg, C. A. Ferguson, & E. A. Moravcsik (Eds.), Universals of human language (Vol. 31, pp. 249–295). Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Gu, Y., Zheng, Y., & Swerts, M. (2019). Having a different pointing of view about the future: The effect of signs on temporal gestures in Mandarin-CSL bimodal bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22 (4), 836–847. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gullberg, M. & Narasimhan, B. (2010). What gestures reveal about the development of semantic distinctions in Dutch children’s placement verbs. Cognitive Linguistics, 21 (2), 239–262. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harrison, S. (2014). The organisation of kinesic ensembles associated with negation. Gesture, 14 (2), 117–140. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haspelmath, M. (1997). Indefinite pronouns. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Healy, C. (2012). Pointing to show agreement. Semiotica, 1921, 175–195.Google Scholar
Hewes, G. W. (1974). Gesture language in culture contact. Sign Language Studies, 4 (1), 1–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huffman, R. (1931). Nuer customs and folklore. London: International Institute of African Language and Culture.Google Scholar
Inbar, A. & Shor, L. (2019). Covert negation in Israeli Hebrew: Evidence from co-speech gestures. Journal of Pragmatics, 1431, 85–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jack, R. E., Garrod, O. G. B., Yu, H., Caldara, R., & Schyns, P. G. (2012). Facial expressions of emotion are not culturally universal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109 (19), 7241–7244. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1972). Motor signs for ‘yes’ and ‘no’. Language in Society, 1 (1), 91–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jehoul, A., Brône, G., & Feyaerts, K. (2017). The shrug as marker of obviousness. Linguistics Vanguard, 3:20160082. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, H. G., Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Communicative body movements: American emblems. Semiotica, 15 (4), 335–353. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jurafsky, D. (1996). Universal tendencies in the semantics of the diminutive. Language, 72 (3), 533–578. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kay, P. & Regier, T. (2003). Resolving the question of color naming universals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100 (15), 9085–9089. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. (1984). Did gesture have the happiness to escape the curse at the confusion of Babel? In A. Wolfgang (Ed.), Nonverbal behavior: Perspectives, applications, intercultural insights (pp. 75–114). Lewiston, NY: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
(1988). Sign languages of Aboriginal Australia: Cultural, semiotic and communicative perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(2002). Some uses of the head shake. Gesture, 2 (2), 147–182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2004). Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kita, S. (2009). Cross-cultural variation of speech-accompanying gesture: A review. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24 (2), 145–167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kita, S. & Özyürek, A. (2003). What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal? Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language, 48 (1), 16–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Knowlson, J. R. (1965). The idea of gesture as a universal language in the XVIIth and XVIIIth centuries. Journal of the History of Ideas, 26 (4), 495–508. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Knox, D. (1990). Ideas on gesture and universal languages, c.1550–1650. In John Henry (Ed.), New perspectives on Renaissance thought: Essays in the history of science, education and philosophy: In memory of Charles B. Schmitt (pp. 101–136). London: Duckworth.Google Scholar
LaBarre, W. (1947). The cultural basis of emotions and gestures. Journal of Personality, 16 (1), 49–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ladewig, S. H. (2011). Putting the cyclic gesture on a cognitive basis. CogniTextes. Revue de l’Association française de linguistique cognitive, 61. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ladewig, Silva H. (2014). Recurrent gestures. In C. Müller, A. Cienki, E. Fricke, S. Ladewig, D. McNeill, & J. Bressem (Eds.), Body – language – communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (pp. 1558–1575). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Le Guen, O. (2011a). Modes of pointing to existing spaces and the use of frames of reference. Gesture, 11 (3), 271–307. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011b). Speech and gesture in spatial language and cognition among the Yucatec Mayas. Cognitive Science, 351, 905–938. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Le Guen, O. & Balam, L. I. P. (2012). No metaphorical timeline in gesture and cognition among Yucatec Mayas. Frontiers in Psychology, 3 (August), 1–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Le Guen, O., Petatillo Balam, R., & Kinil Canché, R. (in press). Yucatec Maya multimodal interaction as the basis for Yucatec Maya Sign Language. In O. Le Guen, J. Safar, & M. Coppola (Eds.), Emerging sign languages of the Americas. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.
Levinson, S. C. & Majid, A. (2013). The island of time: Yélî Dnye, the language of Rossel Island. Frontiers in Psychology, 41 (February), 61. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, H. & Cao, Y. (2019). Hands occupied: Chinese farmers use more non-manual pointing than herders. Lingua, 221, 1–9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Brown, P., Callaghan, T., Takada, A., & de Vos, C. (2012). A prelinguistic gestural universal of human communication. Cognitive Science, 361, 698–713. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lucy, J. A. (1997). Linguistic relativity. Annual Review of Anthroplogy, 261, 291–312. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Majid, A., Bowerman, M., Kita, S., Haun, D. B. M., & Levinson, S. C. (2004). Can language restructure cognition? The case for space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8 (3), 108–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mallery, G. (1882). The gesture speech of man. American Annals of the Deaf and Dumb, 27 (2), 69–89.Google Scholar
Mandel, M. (1977). Iconic devices in American Sign Language. In L. Friedman (Ed.), On the other hand: New perspectives on American Sign Language (pp. 57–107). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Mauss, M. (1973 [orig. 1935]). Techniques of the body. Economy and Society, 2 (1), 70–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McClave, E. (2007). Potential cognitive universals: Evidence from head movements in Turkana. In S. Duncan, J. Cassell, & E. T. Levy (Eds.), Gesture and the dynamic dimension of language. Essays in honor of David McNeill (pp. 91–98). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McClave, E., Kim, H., Tamer, R., & Mileff, M. (2007). Head movements in the context of speech in Arabic, Bulgarian, Korean, and African-American Vernacular English. Gesture, 7 (3), 343–390. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mehr, S. A., Singh, M., Knox, D., Ketter, D. M., Pickens-Jones, D., Atwood, S., [&], & Glowacki, L. (2019). Universality and diversity in human song. Science, 3661 (November, eaax0868), 1–17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mesh, K. (2017). Points of comparison: What indicating gestures tell us about the origins of signs in San Juan Quiahije Chatino Sign Language. Doctoral dissertation, The University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
Morris, D., Collett, P., Marsh, P., & O’Schaughnessy, M. (1979). Gestures: Their origins and distribution. London: Jonathan Cape.Google Scholar
Müller, C. (2004). Forms and uses of the palm up open hand: A case of a gesture family? In C. Müller & R. Posner (Eds.), The semantics and pragmatics of everyday gestures (pp. 233–256). Berlin: Weidler.Google Scholar
Nilsson, N. (1920). Primitive time-reckoning. Lund, Sweden: C. W. K. Gleerup.Google Scholar
Nobe, S. (2000). Where do most spontaneous representational gestures actually occur with respect to speech? In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 186–198). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norenzayan, A. & Heine, S. J. (2005). Psychological universals: What are they and how can we know? Psychological Bulletin, 131 (5), 763–84. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Núñez, R. E. & Sweetser, E. (2006). With the future behind them: Convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cognitive Science, 30 (3), 401–450. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nyst, V. (2016). Size and shape depictions in the manual modality: A taxonomy of iconic devices in Adamorobe Sign Language. Semiotica, 2101, 75–104.Google Scholar
Quintilianus, Marcus Fabius. (1922 [orig. 95 C.E.]). The Institutio Oratoria of Quintilian. Translated by Harold Edgeworth Butler, Vol. 41. New York: G. P. Putnam and Sons (The Loeb Classical Library).Google Scholar
Pouw, W., Harrison, S. J., & Dixon, J. A. (2019). Gesture-speech physics: The biomechanical basis for the emergence of gesture-speech synchrony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roseano, P., González, M., Borràs-Comes, J., & Prieto, P. (2014). Communicating epistemic stance: How speech and gesture patterns reflect epistemicity and evidentiality. Discourse Processes, 53 (3), 135–174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sanders, N. & Napoli, D. J. (2016). Reactive effort as a factor that shapes sign language lexicons. Language, 92 (2), 275–297. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sapir, E. (1949 [orig. 1927]). The unconscious patterning of behavior in society. In D. G. Mandelbaum (Ed.), Selected writings of Edward Sapir (pp. 544–559). Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. A. (1984). On some gestures’ relation to talk. In J. M. Atkinson & J. Heritage (Eds.), Structures of social action (pp. 266–298). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sherzer, J. (1973). Verbal and nonverbal deixis: The pointed lip gesture among the San Blas Cuna. Language in Society, 21, 117–131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sinha, C., Sinha, V. D. S., Zinken, J., & Sampaio, W. (2011). When time is not space: The social and linguistic construction of time intervals and temporal event relations in an Amazonian culture. Language and Cognition, 3 (1), 137–169. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stocking, G. W. (1995). Delimiting anthropology: Historical reflections on the boundaries of a boundless discipline. Social Research, 62 (4), 932–966.Google Scholar
Sullivan, K. & Bui, L. T. (2016). With the future coming up behind them: Evidence that time approaches from behind in Vietnamese. Cognitive Linguistics, 27 (2), 205–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sweetser, E. (1990). From etymology to pragmatics: Metaphorical and cultural aspects of semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tylor, E. (1878). Researches into the early history of mankind. London: John Murray. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vandenabeele, B. (2002). No need for essences. On non-verbal communication in first inter-cultural contacts. South African Journal of Philosophy, 21 (2), 85–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Walker, E. & Cooperrider, K. (2016). The continuity of metaphor: Evidence from temporal gestures. Cognitive Science, 401, 481–495. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilkins, D. (2003). Why pointing with the index finger is not a universal (in sociocultural and semiotic terms). In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 171–215). Mahwah, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cited by (19)

Cited by 19 other publications

Brookes, Heather
2024. Variation in Gesture: A Sociocultural Linguistic Perspective. In The Cambridge Handbook of Gesture Studies,  pp. 616 ff. DOI logo
Gawne, Lauren & Kensy Cooperrider
2024. Emblems: Meaning at the interface of language and gesture. Glossa: a journal of general linguistics 9:1 DOI logo
Goldin‐Meadow, Susan
2024. The Mind Hidden in Our Hands. Topics in Cognitive Science DOI logo
Harrison, Simon
2024. On Grammar–Gesture Relations: Gestures Associated with Negation. In The Cambridge Handbook of Gesture Studies,  pp. 446 ff. DOI logo
Henderson, Mathew, Patrick G. Grosz, Kirsty E. Graham, Catherine Hobaiter & Pritty Patel‐Grosz
2024. Shared semantics: Exploring the interface between human and chimpanzee gestural communication. Mind & Language 39:4  pp. 454 ff. DOI logo
Ladewig, Silva H.
2024. Recurrent Gestures: Cultural, Individual, and Linguistic Dimensions of Meaning-Making. In The Cambridge Handbook of Gesture Studies,  pp. 32 ff. DOI logo
Noland, Carrie
2024. Phenomenology and its phantoms: Maurice Merleau-Ponty and Michel Leiris. French Cultural Studies 35:3  pp. 229 ff. DOI logo
Patel-Grosz, Pritty, Matthew Henderson, Patrick Georg Grosz, Kirsty Graham & Catherine Hobaiter
2024. Primate origins of discourse-managing gestures: the case of hand fling . Linguistics Vanguard 9:1  pp. 63 ff. DOI logo
Li, Heng
2023. Personality in your hands: How extraversion traits influence preference for pointing in Chinese people. Australian Journal of Linguistics 43:2  pp. 121 ff. DOI logo
Littlemore, Jeannette
2023. What Have Bees, Macaque Monkeys, and Humans Got in Common? Embodied Cognition, Gesture, and Second Language Learning. In Applying Cognitive Linguistics to Second Language Learning and Teaching,  pp. 171 ff. DOI logo
Cartmill, Erica A.
2022. Gesture. Annual Review of Anthropology 51:1  pp. 455 ff. DOI logo
Fay, Nicolas, Bradley Walker, T. Mark Ellison, Zachary Blundell, Naomi De Kleine, Murray Garde, Casey J. Lister & Susan Goldin-Meadow
2022. Gesture is the primary modality for language creation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 289:1970 DOI logo
Graham, Kirsty E., Gal Badihi, Alexandra Safryghin, Charlotte Grund & Catherine Hobaiter
2022. A socio-ecological perspective on the gestural communication of great ape species, individuals, and social units. Ethology Ecology & Evolution 34:3  pp. 235 ff. DOI logo
Mustafa, Sriyanti, Baharullah & Vernita Sari
2022. 4TH INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE ON FRONTIERS OF BIOLOGICAL SCIENCES AND ENGINEERING (FBSE 2021) [4TH INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE ON FRONTIERS OF BIOLOGICAL SCIENCES AND ENGINEERING (FBSE 2021), 2511],  pp. 020019 ff. DOI logo
Vandenitte, Sébastien
2022. Making Referents Seen and Heard Across Signed and Spoken Languages: Documenting and Interpreting Cross-Modal Differences in the Use of Enactment. Frontiers in Psychology 13 DOI logo
Bressem, Jana & Claudia Wegener
2021. Handling talk. Gesture 20:2  pp. 219 ff. DOI logo
Harrison, Simon & Silva H. Ladewig
Harrison, Simon, Silva H. Ladewig & Jana Bressem
2021. The diversity of recurrency. Gesture 20:2  pp. 143 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 15 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.