Article published In:
Gesture
Vol. 21:2/3 (2022) ► pp.296319
References (56)
References
Aguasvivas, J. A., Carreiras, M., Brysbaert, M., Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., & Duñabeitia, J. A. (2018). SPALEX: A Spanish lexical decision database from a massive online data collection. Frontiers in Psychology, 9 1, 2156. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M. W. (2005). Gesture in spatial cognition: Expressing, communicating, and thinking about spatial information. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 5 1, 307–331. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Heath, D. C., & Myers, H. J. (2001). Effects of visibility between speaker and listener on gesture production: Some gestures are meant to be seen. Journal of Memory and Language, 44 (2), 169–188. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. E., & Holcomb, P. J. (1995). Auditory and visual semantic priming using different stimulus onset asynchronies: An event-related brain potential study. Psychophysiology, 32 (2), 177–190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59 1, 390–412. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68 (3), 255–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L. W., Santos, A., Simmons, W. K., & Wilson, C. D. (2008). Language and simulation in conceptual processing. In M. de Vega, A. Glenberg, & A. Graesser. (Eds.), Symbols and embodiment: Debates on meaning and cognition (pp. 245–283). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, D. M. (2005). Fitting linear mixed models in R. R News, 5 1, 27–30.Google Scholar
Bates, D. & Maechler, B. (2009). Lme 4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999375-27.Google Scholar
Beattie, G. & Shovelton, H. (2002). An experimental investigation of some properties of individual iconic gestures that mediate their communicative power. British Journal of Psychology, 93 1, 179–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bernardis, P., Salillas, E., & Caramelli, N. (2008). Behavioural and neuropsychological evidence of semantic interaction between iconic gestures and words. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25 1, 1114–1128. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Marco, D., De Stefani, E., & Gentilucci, M. (2015). Gesture and word analysis: the same or different processes? Neuroimage, 117 1, 375–385. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Driskell, J. E. & Radtke, P. H. (2003). The effect of gesture on speech production and comprehension. Human Factors, 45 (3), 445–454. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Forster, K. I. (1981). Priming and the effects of sentence and lexical contexts on naming time: Evidence for autonomous lexical processing. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33 (4), 465–495. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. Center for the Study of Reading Technical Report, No. 257.Google Scholar
Goh, W. D., Suárez, L., Yap, M. J., & Tan, S. H. (2009). Distributional analyses in auditory lexical decision: Neighborhood density and word-frequency effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16 (5), 882–887. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. & Wagner, S. M. (2005). How our hands help us learn. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9 (5), 234–241. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, M. H. & Goodwin, C. (1986). Gesture and coparticipation in the activity of searching for a word. Semiotica, 62 1, 51–76. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Habets, B., Kita, S., Shao, Z., Özyürek, A., & Hagoort, P. (2011). The role of synchrony and ambiguity in speech–gesture integration during comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23 (8), 1845–1854. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hernández-Cabrera, J. A. (2011). ULLRToolbox. [URL]
Holcomb, P. J. & Anderson, J. E. (1993). Cross-modal semantic priming: A time-course analysis using event-related brain potentials. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8 (4), 379–411. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Holcomb, P. J. & Neville, H. J. (1990). Auditory and visual semantic priming in lexical decision: A comparison using event-related brain potentials. Language and Cognitive Processes, 5 (4), 281–312. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hostetter, A. B. (2011). When do gestures communicate? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 137 (2), 297–315. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hubbard, A. L., Wilson, S. M., Callan, D. E., & Dapretto, M. (2009). Giving speech a hand: Gesture modulates activity in auditory cortex during speech perception. Human brain mapping, 30 (3), 1028–1037. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jonides, J. & Mack, R. (1984). On the cost and benefit of cost and benefit. Psychological Bulletin, 96 1, 29–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelly, S. D., Creigh, P., & Bartolotti, J. (2010). Integrating speech and iconic gestures in a Stroop-like task: evidence for automatic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22 (4), 683–694. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelly, S. D., Manning, S. M., & Rodak, S. (2008). Gesture gives a hand to language and learning: Perspectives from cognitive neuroscience, developmental psychology and education. Language and Linguistics Compass, 2 (4), 569–588. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelly, S. D., Özyürek, A., & Maris, E. (2010). Two sides of the same coin: Speech and gesture mutually interact to enhance comprehension. Psychological Science, 21 (2), 260–267. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelly, S., Healey, M., Özyürek, A., & Holler, J. (2015). The processing of speech, gesture, and action during language comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22 (2), 517–523. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. (1980). Gesticulation and speech: Two aspects of the process of utterance. In M. R. Key. (Ed.), The relationship of verbal and nonverbal communication (pp. 207–227). The Hague, the Netherlands: Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1994). Do gestures communicate? A review. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 27 (3), 175–200. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kita, S. & Öyürek, A. (2003). What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal? Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language, 48 1, 16–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krahmer, E. & Swerts, M. (2007). The effects of visual beats on prosodic prominence: Acoustic analyses, auditory perception and visual perception. Journal of Memory and Language, 57 (3), 396–414. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krauss, R. M. (1998). Why do we gesture when we speak? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7 1, 54–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krauss, R. M., Dushay, R. A., Chen, Y., & Rauscher, F. (1995). The communicative value of conversational hand gestures. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 31 1, 533–552. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krauss, R. M., Morrel-Samuels, P., & Colasante, C. (1991). Do conversational hand gestures communicate? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61 1, 743–754. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krauss, R. M., Chen, Y., & Gottesman, R. F. (2000). Lexical gestures and lexical access: a process model. In D. McNeill. (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 261–283). New York: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martuzzi, R., van der Zwaag, W., Farthouat, J., Gruetter, R., & Blanke, O. (2014). Human finger somatotopy in areas 3b, 1, and 2: a 7T fMRI study using a natural stimulus. Human Brain Mapping, 35 (1), 213–226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. (1985). So you think gestures are nonverbal? Psychological Review, 92 (3), 350–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(2005). Gesture and thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morrel-Samuels, P. & Krauss, R. M. (1992). Word familiarity predicts temporal asynchrony of hand gestures and speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18 1, 615–623.Google Scholar
Ng, M. M., Goh, W. D., Yap, M. J., Tse, C. S., & So, W. C. (2017). How we think about temporal words: A gestural priming study in English and Chinese. Frontiers in Psychology, 8 1, 974. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perea, M. & Rosa, E. (2002). The effects of associative and semantic priming in the lexical decision task. Psychological Research, 66 (3), 180–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schneider, W., Eschmann, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools, Inc.Google Scholar
Sidhu, D. M., Vigliocco, G., & Pexman, P. M. (2020). Effects of iconicity in lexical decision. Language and Cognition, 12 (1), 164–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1987). Thinking for speaking. In Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 13 1, 435–445. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1996). From ‘‘thought and language’’ to ‘‘thinking for speaking’’. In J. J. Gumperz & S. C. Levinson. (Eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity (pp. 70–96). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
So, W. C., Low, A., Yap, D. F., Kheng, E., & Yap, M. (2013). Iconic gestures prime words: comparison of priming effects when gestures are presented alone and when they are accompanying speech. Frontiers in Psychology, 4 1, 779. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vitevitch, M. S. & Luce, P. A. (1998). When words compete: Levels of processing in perception of spoken words. Psychological Science, 9 (4), 325–329. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, M. & Emmorey, K. (2006). No difference in short-term memory span between sign and speech. Psychological Science – Cambridge, 17 (12), 1093–1094. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, M. & Knoblich, G. (2005). The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. Psychological Bulletin, 131 (3), 460–473. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wu, Y. C. & Coulson, S. (2005). Meaningful gestures: Electrophysiological indices of iconic gesture comprehension. Psychophysiology, 42 1, 654–667. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007a). Iconic gestures prime related concepts: An ERP study. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14 (1), 57–63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007b). How iconic gestures enhance communication: An ERP study. Brain and Language, 101 1, 234–245. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yap, D. F., So, W. C., Melvin Yap, J. M., Tan, Y. Q., & Teoh, R. L. S. (2011). Iconic gestures prime words. Cognitive Science, 35 (1), 171–183. DOI logoGoogle Scholar