Article published In:
Gesture: Online-First ArticlesDo speakers’ gestures affect listeners’ understanding of temporal relationships between events?
This study investigated how the direction and movement of gestures affect mental representations of time. Previous
research by Tversky and Jamalian (2021) demonstrated that gestures influence how people
perceive temporal relationships, with participants’ drawings reflecting the shape (circular or linear) of the gestures they
observed. However, the influence of gesture direction was not examined. In the present study, 50 Japanese adults were exposed to
different types of gestures, including leftward and rightward linear or circular gestures. Participants were then asked to draw
diagrams representing the temporal flow of events. The results showed significant associations between the type of gesture and the
participants’ drawings. Specifically, the direction and form of the gestures — whether leftward or rightward, linear or circular —
significantly influenced how participants depicted temporal relationships. These findings highlight the importance of both gesture
direction and movement in shaping individuals’ mental representations of time.
Article outline
- Background
- Method
- Participants
- Experimental design
- Experimental material
- Experimental equipment
- Procedure
- Results
- Relationship between gesture type and drawing response
- Analysis of diagrams drawn by experimental participants
- Analysis of congruence between the presented gesture and drawn response
- Discussion
- Pattern of responses
- Congruence
- Relationship with story content
- Future directions
-
References
Published online: 30 September 2024
https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.23020.yos
https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.23020.yos
References (20)
Alcaraz Carrión, D. & Valenzuela, J. (2021). Distant
time, distant gesture: speech and gesture correlate to express temporal
distance. Semiotica, 2411, 159–183.
Boroditsky, L., & Ramscar, M. (2002). The
roles of body and mind in abstract thought. Psychological
Science, 13(2), 185–189.
Bergen, B. K., & Lau, T. T. C. (2012). Writing
direction affects how people map space onto time. Frontiers in
psychology, 3, 109, 1–5.
Callizo-Romero, C., Tutnjević, S., Pandza, M., Ouellet, M., Kranjec, A., Ilić, S., Gu, Y., Göksun, T., Chahboun, S., Casasanto, D., & Santiago, J. (2022). Does
time extend asymmetrically into the past and the future? A multitask crosscultural
study. Language and
Cognition, 14(2), 275–302.
Casasanto, D., & Bottini, R. (2014). Spatial
language and abstract concepts. WIREs Cognitive
Science, 51, 139–149.
Casasanto, D., & Jasmin, K. (2012). The
hands of time: Temporal gestures in English speakers. Cognitive
Linguistics, 23(4), 643–674.
Clark, H. H. (1973). Space,
time, semantics, and the child. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive
development and the acquisition of
language (pp. 27–63). New York: Academic Press.
de la Fuente, J., Santiago, J., Román, A., Dumitrache, C., & Casasanto, D. (2014). When
you think about it, your past is in front of you: How culture shapes spatial conceptions of
time. Psychological
Science, 25(9), 1682–1690.
Feist, M. I., & Duffy, S. E. (2023). To
each their own: A review of individual differences and metaphorical perspectives on
time. Frontiers in
Psychology. 141:1213719.
Gu, Y., Mol, L., Hoetjes, H., & Swerts, M. (2017). Conceptual
and lexical effects on gestures: The case of vertical spatial metaphors for time in
Chinese. Language, Cognition and
Neuroscience, 32(8), 1048–1063.
Kugelmass, S. & Lieblich, A. (1979). Impact
of learning to read on directionality in perception: A further cross-cultural analysis. Human
development, 22(6), 406–415.
Miles, L. K., Tan, L., Noble, G. D. (2011). Can
a mind have two time lines? Exploring space-time mapping in Mandarin and English
speakers. Psychonomic Bulletin &
Review, 181, 598–604.
Núñez, R. E., Cooperrider, K., Doan, D., & Wassmann, J. (2012). Contours
of time: Topographic construals of past, present, and future in the Yupno Valley of Papua New
Guinea. Cognition, 124(1), 25–35.
Núñez, R. E., Motz, B. A., & Teuscher, U. (2006). Time
after time: The psychological reality of the ego — and time-reference-point distinction in metaphorical construals of
time. Metaphor and
Symbol,
21
(3), 133–146.
Pagán Cánovas, C., Valenzuela, J., Alcaraz Carrión, D., Olza, I., & Ramscar, M. (2020). Quantifying
the speech-gesture relation with massive multimodal datasets: Informativity in time
expressions. PLOS
ONE 15(6): e0233892.
Sekine, K., Sowden, H., & Kita, S. (2015). The
Development of the ability to semantically integrate information in speech and iconic gesture in
comprehension. Cognitive
Science,
39
(8), 1855–1880.
Stites, L., & Özçalışkan, Ş. (2021). The
time is at hand: literacy predicts changes in children’s gestures about time. Journal of
Psycholinguistic
Research, 501, 967–983.
Tversky, B., & Jamalian, A. (2021). Thinking
tools: gestures change thought about time. Topics in Cognitive
Science, 131, 750–776.