Part of
Why Gesture?: How the hands function in speaking, thinking and communicating
Edited by Ruth Breckinridge Church, Martha W. Alibali and Spencer D. Kelly
[Gesture Studies 7] 2017
► pp. 397412
References
Alibali, Martha W., and Mitchell, Nathan J.
2007 “Teachers’ gestures as a means of scaffolding students’ understanding: Evidence from an early algebra lesson.” Video Research in the Learning Sciences: 349–365.Google Scholar
Alibali, Martha, W. Flevares, Lucia, and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
1997 “Assessing knowledge conveyed in gesture: Do teachers have the upper hand?” Journal of Educational Psychology 89: 183–193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alibali, Martha W., and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
1993 “Gesture-speech mismatch and mechanisms of learning: What the hands reveal about a child's state of mind.” Cognitive Psychology 25: 468–523. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, David F. and Wilcox, Sherman E.
2007The Gestural Origin of Language. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Church, Ruth B., and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
1986 “The mismatch between gesture and speech as an index of transitional knowledge.” Cognition 23 (1): 43–71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam
1980 “Rules and representations.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1): 1–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Congdon, Eliza L., Novack, Miriam, Brooks, Neon, Hemani-Lopez, Naureen, O'Keefe, Lucy and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
Under review). “Better together: Simultaneous presentation of speech and gesture in math instruction supports generalization and retention.”
Cook, Susan. W., Duff, Melissa, and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
Under Review). “Rethinking memory and learning: Gesture as a vehicle for non-declarative knowledge.”
Corballis, Michael C.
2003From Hand to Mouth: The Origins of Language. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Edelman, Gerald M.
1993 “Neural Darwinism: selection and reentrant signaling in higher brain function.” Neuron 10 (2): 115–125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, Michael A., Feenstra, Eliot, Ryon, Emily and McNeill, David
2011 “A Multimodal approach to coding discourse: Collaboration, distributed cognition, and geometric reasoning." International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning 6 (2): 253–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fodor, Jerry A.
1983The Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gallese, Vittorio and Goldman, Alvin
1998 “Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2, no. 12: 493–501. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2003Hearing Gesture: How Our Hands Help Us Think. Cambridge, M.A.: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan, Wagner, Susan C., and Mitchell, Zachary, A.
2009 “Gesturing gives children new ideas about math.” Psychological Science 20, no. 3: 267–272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2015 “Gesture as a window onto communicative abilities: Implications for diagnosis and intervention.” SIG 1 Perspectives on Language Learning and Education 22: 50–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan, Wein, Debra and Chang, Cecilia
1992 “Assessing knowledge through gesture: Using children's hands to read their minds.” Cognition and Instruction 9 (3): 201–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan, and Sandhofer, Catherine M.
1999 “Gestures convey substantive information about a child’s thoughts to ordinary listeners.” Developmental Science 2 (1): 67–74. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan and McNeill, David
1999 “The role of gesture and mimetic representation in making language the province of speech.” In The Descent of Mind, Michael C. Corballis & Stephen Lea (eds), 155–172. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan, and Singer, Melissa
2003 “From children's hands to adults' ears: Gesture's role in the learning process.” Developmental Psychology 39 (3): 509. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan, Nusbaum, Howard, Kelly, Spencer, and Wagner, Susan
2001 “Explaining math: Gesturing lightens the load.” Psychological Science 12: 516–522. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan, McClintock, Martha K., and Wimsatt, William C.
2004 “Solving psychological problems in four dimensions: Heuristics for integrating the social and biological sciences.” Manuscript, University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2014 “How gesture helps children learn language.” In Language in Interaction: Studies in Honor of Eve V. Clark, I. Arnon, M. Tice, C. Kurumada, & B. Estigarribia (eds), 157–171. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hostetter, Autumn B., and Alibali, Martha W.
2008 “Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action.” Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 15 (3): 495–514. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iacoboni, Marco
2009 “Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons.” Annual Review of Psychology 60: 653–670. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelly, Spencer D., Kravitz, Corinne and Hopkins, Michael
2004 “Neural correlates of bimodal speech and gesture comprehension.” Brain and Language 89 (1): 253–260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelly, Spencer D., and Church, Ruth B.
1997 “Can children detect conceptual information conveyed through other children's nonverbal behavior's.” Cognition and Instruction 15 (1): 107–134. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1998 “A comparison between children' and adults' ability to detect conceptual information conveyed through representational gestures.” Child Development 69 (1): 85–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kendon, Adam
1994 “Do gestures communicate?: A review.” Research on Language and Social Interaction 27: 175–200. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krauss, Robert M.
1998 “Why do we gesture when we speak?” Current Directions in Psychological Science 7: 54–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kutas, Marta, Timothy E. Lindamood, and Steven A. Hillyard
1984 “Word expectancy and event-related brain potentials during sentence processing.” Preparatory states and processes (1984): 217–237.Google Scholar
Kutas, Marta, and Hillyard, Steven A.
1980 “Event-related brain potentials to semantically inappropriate and surprisingly large words.” Biological Psychology 11 (2): 99–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNeill, David, Bertenthal, Bennett, Cole, Jonathon and Gallagher, Shaun
2005 “Gesture-first, but no gestures?Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (2): 138–139. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mol, Lisette, and Kita, Sotaro
2012 “Gesture structure affects syntactic structure in speech.” In Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 761–766.Google Scholar
Novack, Miriam A. and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2017. “Gesture as representational action: A paper about function.” Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. DOI logo
Novack, Miriam A., Elizabeth M. Wakefield, and Susan Goldin-Meadow
2016 “What makes a movement a gesture?.” Cognition 146 : 339–348. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Novack, Miriam A., Congdon, Eliza L., Hemani-Lopez, Naureen, and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2014 “From action to abstraction: Using the hands to learn math.” Psychological Science 25 (4): 903–910. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Núñez, Rafael, and Lakoff, George
2005 “The Cognitive Foundations of Mathematics.” Handbook of Mathematical Cognition: 109–124.Google Scholar
Overton, Willis F.
1994 “The arrow of time and the cycle of time: Concepts of change, cognition, and embodiment.” Psychological Inquiry 5 (3): 215–237. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özçalışkan, Şeyda, Lucero, Ché and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2016a “Is seeing gesture necessary to gesture like a native speaker?” Psychological Science, DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ozcaliskan, Seyda, Lucero, Ché. and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2016b “Does language shape silent gesture?” Cognition 148: 10–18, DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Piaget, Jean, and Bärbel Inhelder
1969The Psychology of the Child. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Ping, Raedy, and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2010 “Gesturing saves cognitive resources when talking about non-present objects.” Cognitive Science 34 (4): 602–619. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shanley, Allen, Özyürek, Asli, Kita, Sotaro, Brown, Amanda, Furman, Reyhan, Ishizuka, Tomoko, and Fujii, Mihoko
2007 “Relations between syntactic encoding and co-speech gestures: Implications for a model of speech and gesture production.” Language and Cognitive Processes 22 (8):1212–1236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Siegler, Robert S., and Crowley, Kevin
1991 “The microgenetic method: A direct means for studying cognitive development.” American Psychologist 46 (6): 606–620. http://dx.doi.org/DOI logo
Singer, Melissa A., and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2005 “Children learn when their teacher's gestures and speech differ.” Psychological Science 16 (2): 85–89. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Streeck, Jürgen
2009Gesturecraft: The Manu-Facture of Meaning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tinbergen, Niko
1963 “On aims and methods of ethology.” Zeitschrift fürTierpsychologie, 20: 410–433. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael and Tomasello, Michael
2009Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wagner, Susan, Nusbaum, Howard, and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2004 “Probing the mental representation of gesture: Is handwaving spatial?” Journal of Memory and Language 50: 395–407. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, Lev
1978 “Interaction between learning and development.” Readings on the Development of Children 23 (3): 34–41.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 4 other publications

Huang, Wen, Candace Walkington & Mitchell J. Nathan
2023. Coordinating modalities of mathematical collaboration in shared VR environments. International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning 18:2  pp. 163 ff. DOI logo
Mlakar, Izidor, Matej Rojc, Darinka Verdonik & Simona Majhenič
2021. Can Turn-Taking Highlight the Nature of Non-Verbal Behavior: A Case Study. In Types of Nonverbal Communication [Working Title], DOI logo
Mlakar, Izidor, Darinka Verdonik, Simona Majhenič & Matej Rojc
2019. Towards Pragmatic Understanding of Conversational Intent: A Multimodal Annotation Approach to Multiparty Informal Interaction – The EVA Corpus. In Statistical Language and Speech Processing [Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 11816],  pp. 19 ff. DOI logo
Tu, Xintian, Joshua Danish, Megan Humburg, Mengxi Zhou, Nitasha Mathayas, Noel Enyedy & Tessaly Jen
2023. Understanding young children’s science learning through embodied communication within an MR environment. International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning 18:2  pp. 231 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 12 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.