Part of
Motion and Space across Languages: Theory and applications
Edited by Iraide Ibarretxe-Antuñano
[Human Cognitive Processing 59] 2017
► pp. 257278
References

References

Allen, S., Özyürek, A., Kita, S., Brown, A., Furman, R., Ishizuka, T., & Fujii, M.
2007Language-specific and universal influences in children’s syntactic packaging of Manner and Path: A comparison of English, Japanese, and Turkish. Cognition, 102, 16–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ameka, F. K., & Essegbey, J.
2013Serializing languages: Satellite-framed, verb-framed or neither?. Ghana Journal of Linguistics, 2(1), 19–38.Google Scholar
Aral, İ.
1999İçimden kuslar göçüyor. Istanbul: Can Yayınları.Google Scholar
Auster, P.
1990The music of chance. New York: Viking Press.Google Scholar
Birsel, M.
1997, July 16. [URL].
Boroditsky, L.
2008Do English and Mandarin speakers think differently about time? In B. C. Love, K. McRae, & V. M. Sloutsky (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th annual conference of the cognitive science society (64–70). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L., & Ramscar, M.
2002The roles of body and mind in abstract thought. Psychological Science, 13(2), 185–189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, A., & Chen, J.
2013Construal of manner in speech and gesture in Mandarin, English, and Japanese. Cognitive Linguistics, 24(4), 605–631. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cacciari, C., Bolognini, N., Senna, I., Pellicciari, M. C., Miniussi, C., & Papagno, C.
2011Literal, fictive, and metaphorical motion sentences preserve the motion component of the verb: A TMS study. Brain & Language, 119, 149–157. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cardini, F. E.
2008Manner of motion saliency: An inquiry into Italian. Cognitive Linguistics, 19(4), 533–569. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Jasmin, K.
2012The hands of time: Temporal gestures in English speakers. Cognitive Linguistics, 23(4), 643–674. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Choi, S., & Bowerman, M.
1991Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: The influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns. Cognition, 41(1–3), 83–121. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Choi, S., & Lantolf, J. P.
2008Representation and embodiment of meaning in L2 communication: Motion events in the speech and gesture of advanced L2 Korean and L2 English speakers. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 30(2), 191–224. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chui, K.
2009Linguistic and imagistic representations of motion events. Journal of Pragmatics, 41(9), 1767–1777. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cifuentes-Férez, P.
Emerson, S. N., Özçalışkan, Ş., & Frishkoff, G.
2016Effects of motion type and modality on word learning in English. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(3), 643–67. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gennari, S. P., Sloman, S. A., Malt, B. C., & Fitch, W.
2002Motion events in language and cognition. Cognition, 83(1), 49–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R. W.
1994The poetics of mind: Figurative thought, language, and understanding. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gu, Y., Mol, L., Hoetjes, M., & Swerts, M.
2017Conceptual and lexical effects on gestures: the case of vertical spatial metaphors for time in Chinese. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience. Advanced online publication. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gullberg, M., Hendriks, H., & Hickmann, M.
2008Learning to talk and gesture about motion in French. First Language, 28(2), 200–236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hickmann, M., Taranne, P., & Bonnet, P.
2009Motion in first language acquisition: Manner and path in French and English child language. Journal of Child Language, 36(4), 705–741. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hohenstein, J.
2013Parent-child talk about motion: Links to children’s development of motion event language. First Language, 33(4), 411–425. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hrabic, M., Williamson, R., & Özçalışkan, Ş.
2015Do young children show sensitivity to language-specific gesture patterns in gesture comprehension? Poster presented at the Society for research in child development biennial meeting. Pittsburg, PA, 19–21 March 2015.Google Scholar
Ibarretxe-Antuñano, I.
2004Language typologies in our language use: The case of Basque motion events in adult oral narratives. Cognitive Linguistics, 15(3), 317–349. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
2005Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological science, 16(5), 367–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Katis, D., & Selimis, S.
2005The development of metaphoric motion: Evidence from Greek children's narratives. Proceedings of the Thirty-first Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 31(1), 205–216.Google Scholar
Kita, S., & Özyürek, A.
2003What does crosslinguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal?: Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 16–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kövecses, Z.
2005Metaphor in culture: Universality and variation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M.
1980Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
1999Philosophy in the flesh. The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lauro, L. J. R., Mattavelli, G., Papagno, C., & Tettamanti, M.
2013She runs, the road runs, my mind runs, bad blood runs between us: Literal and figurative motion verbs: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 83, 361–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maguire, M. J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R., Imai, M., Haryu, E., Vanegas, S., & Sanchez-Davis, B.
2010A developmental shift from similar to language-specific strategies in verb acquisition: A comparison of English, Spanish, and Japanese. Cognition, 114(3), 299–319. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. M.
1991Prelinguistic primitives. Proceedings of the Seventeenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 17, 414–425. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1992How to build a baby II: Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review, 99, 587–604. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matlock, T.
2004Fictive motion as cognitive simulation. Memory and Cognition. 32(8), 1389–1400. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2006Depicting fictive motion in drawings. In J. Luchjenbroers (Ed.), Cognitive Linguistics: Investigations across languages, fields, and philosophical boundaries, (67–85). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matlock, T., Ramscar, M., & Boroditsky, L.
2005On the experimental link between spatial and temporal language. Cognitive Science, 29, 655–664 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matlock, T., & Richardson, D. C.
2004Do eye movements go with fictive motion. In K. D. Forbus, D. Gentner, & T. Reiger (Eds.), Proceedings of the annual conference of the cognitive science society, 26, 909–914. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, Y.
1996Subjective motion and English and Japanese verbs. Cognitive Linguistics, 7(2), 183–226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D.
2000Analogic/analytic representations and crosslinguistic differences in thinking for speaking. Cognitive Linguistics, 11(1–2), 43–60.Google Scholar
Mishra, R. K., & Singh, N.
2010Online fictive motion understanding: An eye-movement study with Hindi. Metaphor and Symbol, 25, 144–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Murdoch, I.
1976A severed head. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Núñez, R. E., Motz, B. A., & Teuscher, U.
2006Time after time: The psychological reality of the ego-and time-reference-point distinction in metaphorical construals of time. Metaphor and symbol, 21(3), 133–146. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Núñez, R. E., & Sweetser, E.
2006With the future behind them: Convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cognitive Science, 30, 401–449. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ochsenbauer, A., & Hickmann, M.
2010Children’s verbalizations of motion events in German. Cognitive Linguistics, 21(2), 217–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oh, K.-J.
2003Language, cognition and development: Motion events in English and Korean. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş.
2003‘In a caravanserai with two doors, I am walking day and night’: Metaphors of death and life in Turkish. Cognitive Linguistics, 14(4), 281–320. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2004Encoding the manner, path and ground components of a metaphorical motion event. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 2, 73–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2005aMetaphor meets typology: Ways of moving metaphorically in English and Turkish. Cognitive Linguistics, 16(1), 207–246. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2005bOn learning to draw the distinction between physical and metaphorical motion: Is metaphor an early emerging cognitive and linguistic capacity? Journal of Child Language, 32(2), 291–318. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2007Metaphors we ‘move by’: Children’s developing understanding of metaphorical motion in typologically distinct languages. Metaphor and Symbol, 22(2), 147–168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2009Learning to talk about spatial motion in language-specific ways. In J. Guo, E. Lieven, S. Ervin-Tripp, N. Budwig, K. Nakamura, & Ş. Özçalışkan (Eds.), Crosslinguistic approaches to the psychology of language: Research in the tradition of Dan Isaac Slobin (263–276). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
2015Ways of crossing a spatial boundary in typologically distinct languages. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(2), 485–508. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2016Do gestures follow speech in bilinguals’ description of motion? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(3), 644–653. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş., Gentner, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
2014Do iconic gestures pave the way for children’s early verbs? Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 1143–1162. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
2005Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development. Cognition, 96(3), B101–B113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2009When gesture-speech combinations do and do not index linguistic change. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28(24), 190–217. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2011Is there an iconic gesture spurt at 26 months?. In G. Stam, & M. Ishino (Eds.), Integrating gestures: The interdisciplinary nature of gesture (163–174). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2010Sex differences in language first appear in gesture. Developmental Science, 13(5), 752–760. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş., Goldin-Meadow, S., Gentner, D., & Mylander, C.
2009Does language about similarity foster similarity comparisons in children? Cognition, 112(2), 217–228. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özçalışkan Ş., Lucero, C., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
2016aIs seeing gesture necessary to gesture like a native speaker? Psychological Science, 27(5), 737–747. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2016bDoes language shape silent gesture? Cognition, 148, 10–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2017Blind speakers show language-specific patterns in co-speech gesture but not silent gesture. Cognitive Science. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş., & Slobin, D. I.
1999Learning how to search for the frog: Expression of manner of motion in English, Spanish, and Turkish. In A. Greenhill, H. Littlefield, & C. Tano (Eds.), Proceedings of the 23rd annual Boston University conference on language development. Vol. 2 (163–174). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş., & Slobin. D. I.
2000‘Climb up’ vs. ‘ascend climbing’: Lexicalization choices in expressing motion events with manner and path components. In S. Catherine-Howell, S. A. Fish, & T. K. Lucas (Eds.), Proceedings of the 24th annual Boston University conference on language development. Vol. 2 (558–570). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş., & Slobin, D. I.
2003Codability effects on the expression of manner of motion in English and Turkish. In A. S. Özsoy, M. Nakipoglu-Demiralp, E. Erguvanlı-Taylan, & A. Aksu-Koç, Studies in Turkish linguistics (259–270). Istanbul: Bogaziçi University Press.Google Scholar
Özyürek, A., & Kita, S.
1999Expressing manner and path in English and Turkish: Differences in speech, gesture, and conceptualization. In M. Hahn, & S. Stoness (Ed.), The twenty first annual conference of the cognitive Science Society (507–512). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Özyürek, A., Kita, S., Allen, S., Brown, A., Furman, R., & Ishizuka, T.
2008Development of crosslinguistic variation in speech and gesture: Motion events in English and Turkish. Developmental Psychology, 44(4), 1040–1054. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papafragou, A., Hulbert, J., & Trueswell, J.
2008Does language guide event perception? Evidence from eye movements. Cognition, 108(1), 155–184. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papafragou, A., Massey, C., & Gleitman, L.
2002Shake, rattle, ’n’ roll: The representation of motion in language and cognition. Cognition, 84(2), 189–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2006When English proposes what Greek presupposes: The crosslinguistic encoding of motion events. Cognition, 98(3), B75–B87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pruden, S. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Maguire, M. J., & Meyer, M. A.
2004Foundations of verb learning: Infants form categories of path and manner in motion events. In A. Brugos, L. Micciulla, & C. E. Smith (Eds.), Proceedings of the 28th annual Boston University conference on language development (461–472). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Richardson, D., & Matlock, T.
2007The integration of figurative language and static depictions: An eye movement study of fictive motion. Cognition, 102, 129–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rojo, A., & Valenzuela, J.
2003Fictive motion in English and Spanish. International Journal of English Studies, 3(2), 123–149.Google Scholar
Selimis, S.
2005The encoding of physical motion in child and adult narratives: A comparison of Greek and English. Studies in Greek Linguistics, 25, 534–545.Google Scholar
Saygın, A. P., McCullough, S., Alac, M., & Emmorey, K.
2009Modulation of BOLD response in motion-sensitive lateral temporal cortex by real and fictive motion sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22 (11), 2480–2490. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scott, A.
1989The vertical dimension and time in Mandarin. Australian Journal of Linguistics, 9, 295–314.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I.
2004The many ways to search for a frog: Linguistic typology and the expression of motion events. In S. Strömqvist, & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Relating events in narrative: Typological contextual perspectives (219–257). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I., Bowerman, M., Brown, P., Eisenbeiss, S., & Narasimhan, B.
2011Putting things in places: Developmental consequences of linguistic typology. In J. Bohnemeyer, & E. Pederson (Eds.), Event representation in language and cognition (134–165). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stites, L. J., & Özçalışkan, Ş.
2013aDevelopmental changes in children’s comprehension and explanation of spatial metaphors for time. Journal of Child Language, 40(5), 1123–1137. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stites, L. J., & Özçalışkan, Ş.
2013bTeasing apart the role of cognitive and linguistic factors in children’s metaphorical abilities. Metaphor and Symbol, 28(2), 1–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stites, L. J., & Özçalışkan, Ş.
2015Children’s early comprehension and expression of metaphors for time are shaped by bodily experience. Poster presented at the Society for research in child development biennial meeting. Pittsburg, PA. 19–21 March 2015.Google Scholar
Talmy, L.
1985Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms. In T. Shopen (Ed.), Language typology and lexical description. Vol. 3: Grammatical categories and the lexicon (57–149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
2000Toward a cognitive semantics. Vol. II: Typology and process in concept structuring. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tsai, P. W. L., & Hsieh, S. C. Y.
2013Fictive motion in Chinese and English tourist guidebooks. Canadian Social Science, 9(2), 1–6.Google Scholar
White, S.
October 14 1999[URL].
Wieselman Schulman, B.
2004A crosslinguistic investigation of the speech-gesture relationship in motion event descriptions. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Wilson, N. L.
2005Conceptualizing motion events and metaphorical motion: Evidence from Spanish/English bilinguals. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of California, Santa Cruz.Google Scholar
Zheng, M., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
2002Thought before language: How deaf and hearing children express motion events across cultures. Cognition, 85(2), 145–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zlatev, J., & Yangklang, P.
2004A third way to travel: The place of Thai in motion-event typology. In S. Strömqvist, & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Relating events in narrative: Typological and contextual perspectives (159–190). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 3 other publications

Cappelle, Bert
2020. Chapter 8. Looking into visual motion expressions in Dutch, English, and French. In Broader Perspectives on Motion Event Descriptions [Human Cognitive Processing, 69],  pp. 235 ff. DOI logo
Lewandowski, Wojciech & Şeyda Özçalışkan
2023.  Running across the mind or across the park: does speech about physical and metaphorical motion go hand in hand?. Cognitive Linguistics 34:3-4  pp. 411 ff. DOI logo
Moore, Kevin Ezra
2020. Moving Time vs. Frame-relative motion. Constructions and Frames 12:2  pp. 272 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 12 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.