Part of
Linguistic Approaches to Portuguese as an Additional Language
Edited by Karina Veronica Molsing, Cristina Becker Lopes Perna and Ana Maria Tramunt Ibaños
[Issues in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics 24] 2020
► pp. 6182
References (47)
References
Barbosa, P. A., & Albano, E. C. (2004). Brazilian Portuguese. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 34(2), 227–232. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Best, C. T. (1995). A direct realist perspective on cross-language speech perception. In W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research (pp. 171–204). Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Carvalho, A. M., & Baclear da Silva, A. J. (2006). Cross-linguistic influence in third language acquisition: The case of Spanish-English bilinguals’ acquisition of Portuguese. Foreign Language Annals, 39(2), 185–202. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carvalho, A. M., Freire, J. L., Silva, A. J. B. (2010). Teaching Portuguese to Spanish speakers: A case for trilingualism. Hispania, 93(1), 70–75.Google Scholar
Cox, F. (2006). The acoustic characteristics of /hVd/ vowels in the speech of some Australian teenagers. Australian Journal of Linguistics, 26(2), 147–179. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diaz Granado, M. (2011). L2 and L3 acquisition of the Portuguese stressed vowel inventory by native speakers of English (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Arizona.Google Scholar
Elvin, J. (2016). The role of the native language in non-native perception and spoken word recognition: English vs. Spanish learners of Portuguese (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Western Sydney University.Google Scholar
Elvin, J. & Escudero, P. (2019). Cross-linguistic influence in second language speech: Implications for learning and teaching. In J. Gutierrez-Mangado, M. Martínez Adrián, & F. Gallardo Del Puerto (Eds.), Cross-linguistic influence: From empirical evidence to classroom practice, Dordrecht: Springer. DOI logo
Elvin, J., Escudero, P., & Vasiliev, P. (2014). Spanish is better than English for discriminating Portuguese vowels: Acoustic similarity versus vowel inventory. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(OCT), 1–8. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Elvin, J., Escudero, P., Williams, D., & Best, C. T. (2016). The relationship between Australian English speakers’ non-native perception and production of Brazilian Portuguese vowels. In Proceedings of the 16th Australasian International Conference on Speech Science and Technology, Sydney, Australia.Google Scholar
Elvin, J., Williams, D., & Escudero, P. (2016). The relationship between perception and production of Brazilian Portuguese vowels in European Spanish monolinguals. Loquens, 3(2), e031. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P. (2005). Linguistic perception and second language acquisition (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Utrecht University.Google Scholar
(2006). Second language phonology: The role of perception. In M. Pennington (Ed.), Phonology in context (pp. 109–134). Houndmills: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
(2009). The linguistic perception of similar L2 sounds. Phonology in Perception, 15, 152–190.Google Scholar
Escudero, P., Benders, T., & Wanrooij, K. (2011). Enhanced bimodal distributions facilitate the learning of second language vowels. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130(4), EL206–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P., & Boersma, P. (2002). The subset problem in L2 perceptual development: Multiple-category assimilation by Dutch learners of Spanish. In B. Skarabela, S. Fish, & A. H.-J. Do (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 208–219). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
(2004). Bridging the gap between L2 speech perception research and phonological theory. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26(4), 551–585. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P., Boersma, P., Rauber, A. S., & Bion, R. a H. (2009). A cross-dialect acoustic description of vowels: Brazilian and European Portuguese. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 126(3), 1379–1393. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P., Broersma, M., & Simon, E. (2013). Learning words in a third language: Effects of vowel inventory and language proficiency. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28(6), 746–761. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P., & Chládková, K. (2010). Spanish listeners’ perception of American and Southern British English vowels. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 128(5), EL254–9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P., Hayes-Harb, R., & Mitterer, H. (2008). Novel second-language words and asymmetric lexical access. Journal of Phonetics, 36(2), 345–360. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P., Mulak, K. E., Elvin, J., & Traynor, N. M. (2018). “Mummy, keep it steady”: Phonetic variation shapes word learning at 15 and 17 months. Developmental science, 21(5), e12640. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P., Simon, E., & Mitterer, H. (2012). The perception of English front vowels by North Holland and Flemish listeners: Acoustic similarity predicts and explains cross-linguistic and L2 perception. Journal of Phonetics, 40(2), 280–288. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P., Simon, E., & Mulak, K. (2014). Learning words in a new language: Orthography doesn’t always help. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(2), 384–395. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P., Sisinni, B., & Grimaldi, M. (2014). The effect of vowel inventory and acoustic properties in Salento Italian learners of Southern British English vowels. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 135(3), 1577–1584. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P., & Wanrooij, K. (2010). The effect of L1 orthography on non-native vowel perception. Language and Speech, 53(3), 343–365. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P., & Williams, D. (2012). Native dialect influences second-language vowel perception: Peruvian versus Iberian Spanish learners of Dutch. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 131(5), EL406–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P., & Kalashnikova, M. (2020). Infants use phonetic detail in speech perception and word learning when detail is easy to perceive. Journal of experimental child psychology, 190, 104714. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E. (1995). Second language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research (pp. 233–276). Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., & Bohn, O.-S. (1989). An instrumental study of vowel reduction and stress placement in Spanish-accented English. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 11, 35–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giancaspro, D., Halloran, B., & Iverson, M. (2014). Transfer at the initial stages of L3 Brazilian Portuguese: A look at three groups of English / Spanish bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(2), 191–207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gordon, L. S. (2008). Factors affecting English speakers’ perception of L2 vowels (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Georgetown University.Google Scholar
Henriques, E. (2000). Text intercomprehension by native speakers of Portuguese and Spanish. Delta, 16(2), 263–295. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, P., & Evans, B. G. (2007). Learning English vowels with different first-language vowel systems: Perception of formant targets, formant movement, and duration. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 122(5), 2842–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009). Learning English vowels with different first-language vowel systems II: Auditory training for native Spanish and German speakers. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 126(2), 866–877. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jensen, J. B. (1989). On the mutual intelligibility of Spanish and Portuguese. His, 72(4), 848–852.Google Scholar
Kendall, R. R. (2004). The perception and production of Portuguese mid-vowels by native speakers of American English (Unpublished doctoral disseration). Brigham Young University.Google Scholar
Ladefoged, P., & Johnson, K. (2011). A course in phonetics (6th ed.). Boston, MA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
Maye, J., Werker, J. F., & Gerken, L. (2002). Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition, 82(3), b101–b111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., Dias, R., & Santos, H. (2010). Clitics and object expression in the L3 acquisition of Brazilian Portuguese: Structural similarity matters for transfer. Second Language Research, 27(1), 21–58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morrison, G. S. (2003). Perception and production of Spanish vowels by English speakers. In Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 1533–1536). Barcelona.Google Scholar
(2009). L1-Spanish speakers’ acquisition of the English /i/--/ / Contrast II: Perception of vowel inherent spectral change 1. Language and Speech, 52(4), 437–462. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rauber, A. S., Escudero, P., Bion, R. A. H., & Baptista, B. O. (2005). The interrelation between the perception and production of English vowels by native speakers of Brazilian Portuguese graduate program in applied linguistics. INTERSPEECH, 2, 2913–2916.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. (2010). L3 syntactic transfer selectivity and typological determinacy: The typological primacy model. Second Language Research, 27(1). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shatzman, K., & McQueen, J. (2006). Segment duration as a cue to word boundaries in spoken-word recognition. Perception & Psychophysics, 68(1), 1–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Leussen, J. W., & Escudero, P. (2015). Learning to perceive and recognize a second language: The L2LP model revised. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(August), 1–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vasiliev, P. (2013). The initial state for Californian English learners of Spanish and Portuguese vowels (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Cited by (4)

Cited by four other publications

Liu, Xiaoluan & Paola Escudero
2023. How bidialectalism affects non-native speech acquisition: Evidence from Shanghai and Mandarin Chinese. Applied Psycholinguistics 44:6  pp. 969 ff. DOI logo
Smit, Eline A., Andrew J. Milne & Paola Escudero
2022. Music Perception Abilities and Ambiguous Word Learning: Is There Cross-Domain Transfer in Nonmusicians?. Frontiers in Psychology 13 DOI logo
Mulak, Karen E., Hannah S. Sarvasy, Alba Tuninetti, Paola Escudero & Niels O. Schiller
2021. Word learning in the field: Adapting a laboratory-based task for testing in remote Papua New Guinea. PLOS ONE 16:9  pp. e0257393 ff. DOI logo
Tuninetti, Alba, Karen E. Mulak & Paola Escudero
2020. Cross-Situational Word Learning in Two Foreign Languages: Effects of Native Language and Perceptual Difficulty. Frontiers in Communication 5 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 17 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.