Article published In:
The Unit of Processing in Chinese
Edited by Tianlin Wang
[International Journal of Chinese Linguistics 11:1] 2024
► pp. 5693
References (84)
References
Arnon, I., & Christiansen, M. H. (2017). The Role of Multiword Building Blocks in Explaining L1-L2 Differences. Topics in cognitive science, 9(3), 621–636. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arnon, I., & Clark, E. V. (2011). Why brush your teeth is better than teeth–Children’s word production is facilitated in familiar sentence-frames. Language Learning and Development, 7 (2), 107–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arnon, I., & Cohen-Priva, U. (2013). More than words: The effect of multi-word frequency and constituency on phonetic duration. Language and Speech, 56 1, 349–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arnon, I., & Snider, N. (2010). More than words: Frequency effects for multi-word phrases. Journal of Memory and Language, 62 1, 67–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., & Milin, P. (2010). Analysing reaction times. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3 (2), 12–28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bannard, C., & Matthews, D. (2008). Stored word sequences in language learning: The effect of familiarity on children’s repetition of four-word combinations. Psychological Science, 19 (3), 241–248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bao, Z. 包智明. (2021). 论汉语重音 [Chinese stress]. 韵律语法研究 [Studies in Prosodic Grammar], 8 1, 4–22.Google Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting-linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67 1, 1–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Biber, D., Johansson, S., Leech, G., Conrad, S., Finegan, E., & Quirk, R. (1999). Longman grammar of spoken and written English. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bock, K. (1996). Language production: Methods and methodologies. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3 1, 395–421. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brandt, S., Diessel, H., & Tomasello, M. (2008). The acquisition of German relative clauses: a case study. Journal of Child Language, 35 (2), 325–348. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. (2006). From usage to grammar: The minds response to repetition. Language, 82 1, 711–733. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007). Frequency of Use and the Organization of Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cao, J. 曹剑芬. (1986). 普通话轻声音节特性分析 [An analysis of the characteristics of neutral tone syllables in Mandarin Chinese]. 应用声学 [Journal of Applied Acoustics], 5 (4), 1–6.Google Scholar
Carrol, G., & Conklin, K. (2020). Is All Formulaic Language Created Equal? Unpacking the Processing Advantage for Different Types of Formulaic Sequences. Language and speech, 63 (1), 95–122. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chang, X. 常新茹. (2020). 搭配强度与结构类型对中高级泰国汉语学习者限制性搭配加工的影响 [A study of the influence of collocational strength and structure types on the processing of restricted colocations by intermediate and advanced Chinese learners from Thailand]. 语言教学与研究 [Language Teaching and Linguistic Studies], (03), 22–32.Google Scholar
Chao, Y. (1968). A Grammar of Spoken Chinese. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Chen, Y., & Xu, Y. (2006). Production of weak elements in speech–evidence from f₀ patterns of neutral tone in Standard Chinese. Phonetica, 63 (1), 47–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cheng, S. 程书秋. (2012). 现代汉语短语使用情况的优先序列考察 [An Investigation of Priority Sequences in the Usage of Modern Chinese Phrases]. 汉语学报 [Chinese Linguistics], (02), 91–94.Google Scholar
Conklin, K., & Schmitt, N. (2008). Formulaic sequences: Are they processed more quickly than nonformulaic language by native and nonnative speakers?. Applied linguistics, 29 (1), 72–89. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cop, U., Keuleers, E., Drieghe, D., & Duyck, W. (2015). Frequency effects in monolingual and bilingual natural reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22 1, 1216–1234. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diependaele, K., Lemhöfer, K., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). The word frequency effect in first-and second-language word recognition: A lexical entrenchment account. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66 (5), 843–863. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Durrant, P., & Doherty, A. (2010). Are high-frequency collocations psychologically real? Investigating the thesis of collocational priming. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 6 (2), 125–155.
Ellis, N. C., Simpson-Vlach, R. I. T. A., & Maynard, C. (2008). Formulaic language in native and second language speakers: Psycholinguistics, corpus linguistics, and TESOL. Tesol Quarterly, 42 (3), 375–396. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Estes, K. G., & Lew-Williams, C. (2015). Listening through voices: Infant statistical word segmentation across multiple speakers. Developmental psychology, 51 (11), 1517–1528. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fang, Y., & Jiang, X. 房艳霞, 江新. (2020). 视觉输入增强对汉语二语学习者语块学习的影响 [The impact of visual input enhancement on Chinese as a second language learners’ chunk learning]. 语言教学与研究 [Language Teaching and Linguistic Studies], (05), 41–52.Google Scholar
Gablasova, D., Brezina, V. and McEnery, T. (2017). Collocations in Corpus-Based Language Learning Research: Identifying, Comparing, and Interpreting the Evidence. Language Learning, 67 1, 155–179. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. (2006). Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
González Fernández, B., & Schmitt, N. (2015). How much collocation knowledge do L2 learners have? The effects of frequency and amount of exposure. ITL-international journal of applied linguistics, 166 (1), 94–126. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gries, S. T., & Ellis, N. C. (2015). Statistical measures for usage-based linguistics. Language Learning, 65 (S1), 228–255. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grimm, R., Cassani, G., Gillis, S., & Daelemans, W. (2019). Children probably store short rather than frequent or predictable chunks: Quantitative evidence from a corpus study. Frontiers in psychology, 10 1. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gyllstad, H., & Wolter, B. (2016). Collocational processing in light of the phraseological continuum model: Does semantic transparency matter? Language Learning, 66 (2), 296–323.Google Scholar
Han, S. (2019). Frequency, MI, and Congruency in Collocation Processing by Korean EFL Learners: Evidence from Reading Aloud. 영어학, 19 (3), 325–346.Google Scholar
Hay, J., Wang, T., & Saffran, J. (2012). Perceptual narrowing in the second year: the case of pitch contour. In 37th Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, MA.
Hernández, M., Costa, A., & Arnon, I. (2016). More than words: multiword frequency effects in non-native speakers. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31 (6), 785–800.Google Scholar
Hou, X., Sun, P., & Zhang, T. 侯晓明, 孙培健, 张婷婷. (2022). 整体频次和搭配强度在母语和二语多词序列加工中的作用:以汉语双字副词短语为例 [The role of overall frequency and collocation strength in processing multi-word sequences in L1 and L2 Chinese: The case of bisyllabic adverb phrases]. 世界汉语教学 [Chinese Teaching in the World], 36 (2), 236–250.Google Scholar
Howarth, P. (1998). Phraseology and second language proficiency. Applied Linguistics, 19 (1), 24–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hu, L., & Tang, X. (2014). Multiword Expression Extraction Based on Word Relativity. International Journal of Knowledge and Language Processing, 5 (1), 27–40.Google Scholar
Huang, L. (2001). Knowledge of English collocations: An analysis of Taiwanese EFL learners. In C. Luke & B. Rubrecht (Eds.), Texas Papers in Foreign Language Education: Selected Proceedings from the Texas Foreign Language Education Conference, 2001 (Vol. 6, No. 1, pp. 21).Google Scholar
Hunston, S. (2002). Corpora in applied linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61 (4), 343–65. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jiang, S., Jiang, X., & Siyanova-Chanturia, A. (2020). The processing of multiword expressions in children and adults: An eye-tracking study of Chinese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1 1, 1–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jolsvai, H., McCauley, S. M., & Christiansen, M. H. (2020). Meaningfulness beats frequency in multiword chunk processing. Cognitive Science, 44 (10), e12885. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jones, G., Cabiddu, F., & Avila-Varela, D. S. (2020). Two-year-old children’s processing of two-word sequences occurring 19 or more times per million and their influence on subsequent word learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 199 1, 104922. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kerz, E., Wiechmann, D., Frinsel, F., & Christiansen, M. H. (2020). Tuning to Real-Life Language Statistics: Online Processing of Multiword Sequences in Native and Non-Native Speakers Across Language Registers. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Lexical frequency and exemplar-based learning effects in language acquisition: evidence from sentential complements. Language Sciences, 32 (1), 132–142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, H., Warrington, K. L., Pagán, A., Paterson, K. B., & Wang, X. (2021). Independent effects of collocation strength and contextual predictability on eye movements in reading. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 36 (8), 1001–1009.Google Scholar
Li, Y. 李宇明. (2004). 儿童语言的发展 [The Development of Children’s Language], Wuhan: Central China Normal University Press.Google Scholar
Matthews, D., & Bannard, C. J. (2010). Children’s Production of Unfamiliar Word Sequences Is Predicted by Positional Variability and Latent Classes in a Large Sample of Child-Directed Speech. Cognitive Science, 34(3), 465–488. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McCauley, S. M., & Christiansen, M. H. (2017). Computational investigations of multiword chunks in language learning. Topics in Cognitive Science, 9 (3), 637–652. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Naigles, L. R., & Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1998). Why are some verbs learned before other verbs? Effects of input frequency and structure on children’s early verb use. Journal of Child Language, 25 1, 95–120. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nattinger, J. R., & DeCarrico, J. S. (1992). Lexical phrases and language teaching. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Öksüz, D., Brezina, V., & Rebuschat, P. (2021). Collocational Processing in L1 and L2: The effects of word frequency, collocational frequency, and association. Language Learning, 71 (1), 55–98. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S., & Ullman, M. T. (2002). The past and future of the past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6 1, 456–463. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prévot, L., Tseng, S. C., Peshkov, K., & Chen, A. C. H. (2015). Processing units in conversation: A comparative study of French and Mandarin data. Language and Linguistics, 16 (1), 69–92.Google Scholar
Qian, X. 钱旭菁. (2008). 汉语语块研究初探 [A preliminary study of Chinese language chunks]. 北京大学学报(哲学社会科学版) [Journal of Peking University (Philosophy & Social Sciences)], (05), 139–146.Google Scholar
Rauzy, S., & Blache, P. (2012, December). Robustness and processing difficulty models. a pilot study for eye-tracking data on the french treebank. In Proceedings of the First Workshop on Eye-tracking and Natural Language Processing (pp. 21–36).Google Scholar
Riches, N., Letts, C., Hadeel, A. W. A. D., Ramsey, R., & Dąbrowska, E. (2021). Collocational knowledge in children: a comparison of English-speaking monolingual children, and children acquiring English as an Additional Language. Journal of Child Language, 1–16.Google Scholar
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274 1, 1926–1928. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shi, F., & Xia, Q. 石锋, 夏全胜. (2022). 试论汉语普通话语句的焦点原理 [The principles of sentence focus in Mandarin Chinese]. 语言科学 [Language Science], 21 (6), 580–594.Google Scholar
Siyanova-Chanturia, A., & Janssen, N. (2018). Production of familiar phrases: Frequency effects in native speakers and second language learners. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44 (12), 2009.Google Scholar
Skarabela, B., Ota, M., O’Connor, R., & Arnon, I. (2021). ‘Clap your hands’ or ‘take your hands’? One-year-olds distinguish between frequent and infrequent multiword phrases. Cognition, 211 1, 104612. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, S. A., & Murphy, V. A. (2015). Measuring productive elements of multi-word phrase vocabulary knowledge among children with English as an additional or only language. Reading and Writing, 28 (3), 347–369.Google Scholar
Sonbul, S. (2015). Fatal mistake, awful mistake, or extreme mistake? Frequency effects on off-line/on-line collocational processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18 1, 419–437. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sosa, A. V., & MacFarlane, J. (2002). Evidence for frequency-based constituents in the mental lexicon: Collocations involving the word of. Brain and Language, 83 (2), 227–236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stubbs, M. (1995). Collocations and semantic profiles: On the cause of the trouble with quantitative studies. Functions of Language, 2 (1), 23–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tremblay, A., & Baayen, R. H. (2010). Holistic processing of regular four-word sequences: A behavioral and ERP study of the effects of structure, frequency, and probability on immediate free recall. In D. Wood (Ed.), Perspectives on formulaic language: acquisition and communication (pp. 151–173). London, UK: Continuum.Google Scholar
Tremblay, A., Derwing, B., Libben, G., & Westbury, C. (2011). Processing advantages of lexical bundles: Evidence from self-paced reading and sentence recall tasks. Language Learning, 61 1, 569–613. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wang, J., & Wang, X. 王娟, 汪鑫鑫. (2021). 母亲语言支架预测儿童亲社会行为:儿童情绪理解的中介作用 [Maternal Verbal Scaffolding Positively Predicts Children’s Prosocial Behavior: The Mediating Role of Children’s Emotional Understanding]. 中国临床心理学杂志 [Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology], 29(01), 19–23.Google Scholar
Wang, S. (2020). Chinese Multiword Expressions: Theoretical and Practical Perspectives. Singapore: Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wang, T., Potter, C. E., & Saffran, J. R. (2020). Plasticity in second language learning: The case of Mandarin tones. Language Learning and Development, 16 (3), 231–243. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wolter, B., & Gyllstad, H. (2013). Frequency of input and L2 collocational processing: A comparison of congruent and incongruent collocations. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35 (3), 451–482. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wray, A. (2002). Formulaic language and the lexicon. UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Xu, Y., & Wang, T. 许莹莹, 王同顺. (2015). 频率、一致性及水平对二语语块加工的影响 [Effects of frequency, congruency and proficiency on the processing of L2 formulaic sequences]. 现代外语 [Modern Foreign Languages], 38(03), 376–385.Google Scholar
Xue, X., & Shi, C. 薛小芳, 施春宏. (2013). 语块的性质及汉语语块系统的层级关系 [The nature of language chunks and the hierarchical relationship in the Chinese language chunk system]. 当代修辞学 [Contemporary Rhetoric], (03), 32–46.Google Scholar
Yamashita, J., & Jiang, N. A. N. (2010). L1 influence on the acquisition of L2 collocations: Japanese ESL users and EFL learners acquiring English collocations. Tesol Quarterly, 44 (4), 647–668. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yi, W. (2018). Statistical sensitivity, cognitive aptitudes, and processing of collocations. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40 (4), 831–856. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yi, W., Lu, S., & Ma, G. (2017). Frequency, contingency and online processing of multiword sequences: An eye-tracking study. Second Language Research, 33 1, 519–549. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yu, W., Wang, L., Qu, X., Wang, T., Zhang, J., & Liang, D. 于文勃, 王璐, 瞿邢芳, 王天琳, 张晶晶, 梁丹丹. (2021). 转换概率和词长期待对语音统计学习的影响 [Transitional probabilities and expectation for word length impact verbal statistical learning]. 心理学报 [Acta Psychologica Sinica], 53 (6), 565–574.Google Scholar
Zhang, P., & Fang, N. 张萍, 方南. (2020). 词频、语义和语言水平对英语搭配加工的影响 [Effects of lexical frequency, semantic congruency, and language proficiency on English collocational processing by Chinese EFL learners]. 外语教学与研究 [Foreign Language Teaching and Research], 52 (04), 532–545.Google Scholar
Zhang, X., & Zhang, X. 张晓鹏, 张晓瑾. (2020). 频率和方向偏好对英语verb+noun搭配加工的影响研究 [Effects of Directionality and Frequency on the Processing of English Collocations]. 外语与外语教学 [Foreign Languages and Their Teaching], (02), 16–26.Google Scholar
Zheng, H., Li, H., & Wang, Y. 郑航, 李慧, 王一一. (2016). 语境中语块的加工及其影响因素:以中级汉语学习者为例 [Processing of language chunks in context and its influencing factors: A case study of intermediate Chinese learners]. 世界汉语教学 [Chinese Teaching in the World], 30 (03), 401–418.Google Scholar
Zhou, J. 周健. (2007). 语块在对外汉语教学中的价值与作用 [The value and role of language chunks in teaching Chinese as a foreign language]. 暨南学报(哲学社会科学版) [Jinan Journal (Philosophy & Social Sciences)], (01), 99–104.Google Scholar
Cited by (1)

Cited by one other publication

Wang, Tianlin
2024. Introduction. International Journal of Chinese Linguistics 11:1  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 4 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.