Turn structure and inserts
Turns-at-talk often do not start with their main business but rather with a pre-start (Sacks et al., 1974). This paper investigates the correlation of pre-starts with inserts, one of three major word classes (Biber et al., 1999). Based on the BNC’s mark-up, I investigate how inserts are positionally
distributed in large amounts of turns of varied lengths. The analysis shows that inserts are overwhelmingly attracted to turn-first
positions, the likely location of pre-starts. Further, in a subsample of 1,000 ten-word turns manually coded for pre-starts, 86% of all
inserts serve a pre-start function. The findings call into question current speech processing models that fail to factor in turn structure.
Further, pre-starts have crucial sequential and interactional implications as early indicators whether the new turn “agrees” with the prior
turn and are likely key signals aiding listeners’ action ascription.
Keywords: interjection, contextualization cue, textual colligation, speech planning, action ascription
Published online: 28 August 2020
https://doi.org/10.1075/ijcl.19098.ruh
https://doi.org/10.1075/ijcl.19098.ruh
References
References
Aijmer, K.
Aijmer, K., & Rühlemann, C.
Ameka, F.
Biber, D., Johansson, S., Leech, G., Conrad, S., & Finegan, E.
[ p. 210 ]
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D.
(2012) Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer (Version 6.0.37) [Computer software]. http://www.praat.org/
Bögels, S., Kendrick, K. H., & Levinson, S. C.
Bögels, S., Magyari, L., & Levinson, S. C.
Brinton, L. J.
Buysse, L.
Carter, R. A., Hughes, R., & McCarthy, M. J.
Clayman, S. E.
Couper-Kuhlen, E., & Selting, M.
Crystal, D.
Damian, M. F., Vigliocco, G., & Levelt, W. J. M.
Diani, G.
Evison, J.
Gisladottir, R. S., Bögels, S., & Levinson, S. C.
Gumperz, J. J.
Heldner, M., & Edlund, J.
Heritage, J., & Sorjonen, M. L.
Hoffmann, S., Evert, S., Smith, N., Lee, D., & Berglund Prytz, Y.
Holler, J., Kendrick, K. H., & Levinson, S. C.
Indefrey, P., & Levelt, W. J. M.
Jefferson, G.
Jescheniak, J. D., & Levelt, W. J. M.
Jongman, S. R., Piai, V., & Meyer, A. S.
Jucker, A. H.
De Klerk, V.
Koivisto, A.
Levelt, W. J. M., Praamstra, P., Meyer, A. S., Helenius, P., & Salmelin, R.
Levelt, W. J., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S.
Levinson, S. C.
Levinson, S. C., & Holler, J.
Levinson, S. C., & Torreira, F.
Magyari, L., Bastiaansen, M. C. M., de Ruiter, J. P., & Levinson, S. C.
Mehl, M. R., Vazire, S., Ramirez-Esparza, N., Slatcher, R. B., & Pennebaker, J. W.
Ochs, E., Schegloff, E. A., & Thompson, S. A.
Oldfield, R. C., & Wingfield, A.
Pfeiffer, M.
Pomerantz, A., & Heritage, J.
Rayson, P., Leech, G., & Hodges, M.
Roberts, S. G., Torreira, F., & Levinson, S. C.
Roberts, S. G., & Levinson, S. C.
Rochmont, M., & Cullicover, P.
Romero-Trillo, J.
Rühlemann, C.
Rühlemann, C., & Gee, M.
Rühlemann, C., & Schweinberger, M.
under review). Information in turns-at-talk: Modeling the placement of nuclear stress.
Sacks, H.
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G.
Schegloff, E. A.
Schegloff, E. A.
Schegloff, E. A., & Lerner, G. H.
Sicoli, M., Stivers, T., Enfield, N. J., & Levinson, S. C.
Stivers, T., Enfield, N. J., Brown, P., Englert, C., Hayashi, M., Heinemann, T., Hoymann, G., Rossano, F., de Ruiter, J. P., Yoon, K.-E., & Levinson, S. C.
Svartvik, J.
Tao, H.
Torreira, F., Bögels, S. & Levinson, S. C.
Tottie, G.
Vaughan, E., McCarthy, M., & Clancy, B.
[ p. 214 ]
Yuan, J., M. Liberman, & Cieri, C.
(2006) Towards an integrated understanding of speaking rate in conversation. In Interspeech 2006 – ICSLP (pp. 541–544). Pittsburgh, PA. https://www.isca-speech.org/archive/archive_papers/interspeech_2006/i06_1795.pdf