Turns-at-talk often do not start with their main business but rather with a pre-start (Sacks et al., 1974). This paper investigates the correlation of pre-starts with inserts, one of three major word classes (Biber et al., 1999). Based on the BNC’s mark-up, I investigate how inserts are positionally
distributed in large amounts of turns of varied lengths. The analysis shows that inserts are overwhelmingly attracted to turn-first
positions, the likely location of pre-starts. Further, in a subsample of 1,000 ten-word turns manually coded for pre-starts, 86% of all
inserts serve a pre-start function. The findings call into question current speech processing models that fail to factor in turn structure.
Further, pre-starts have crucial sequential and interactional implications as early indicators whether the new turn “agrees” with the prior
turn and are likely key signals aiding listeners’ action ascription.
Aijmer, K. (2013). Understanding Pragmatic Markers: A Variational Pragmatic Approach. Edinburgh University Press.
Aijmer, K., & Rühlemann, C. (Eds.) (2015). Corpus Pragmatics: A Handbook. Cambridge University Press.
Ameka, F. (1992). Interjections: The universal yet neglected part of speech. Journal of Pragmatics, 18(2–3), 101–118.
Biber, D., Johansson, S., Leech, G., Conrad, S., & Finegan, E. (1999). Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English. Pearson Education.
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2012). Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer (Version 6.0.37) [Computer software]. [URL]
Bögels, S., Kendrick, K. H., & Levinson, S. C. (2015). Never say no … How the brain interprets the pregnant pause in conversation. PLoS ONE, 10(12), e0145474.
Bögels, S., Magyari, L., & Levinson, S. C. (2015). Neural signatures of response planning occur midway through an incoming question in conversation. Scientific Reports, 5(12881), 1–11.
Brinton, L. J. (2010). Discourse markers. In A. H. Jucker & I. Taavitsainen (Eds.), Historical Pragmatics (pp. 285–314). De Gruyter Mouton.
Buysse, L. (2012). So as a multifunctional discourse marker in native and learner speech. Journal of Pragmatics, 44(13), 1764–1782.
Carter, R. A., Hughes, R., & McCarthy, M. J. (2000). Exploring Grammar in Context. Cambridge University Press.
Clayman, S. E. (2013). Turn-constructional units and the transition-relevance place. In J. Sidnell & T. Stivers (Eds.), The Handbook of Conversation Analysis (pp. 150–166). Wiley Blackwell.
Couper-Kuhlen, E., & Selting, M. (2017). Interactional Linguistics: An Introduction to Language in Social Interaction. Cambridge University Press.
Crystal, D. (2003). The Cambridge Encyclopedia of the English Language (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press.
Damian, M. F., Vigliocco, G., & Levelt, W. J. M. (2001). Effects of semantic context in the naming of pictures and words. Cognition, 811, B77–B86.
Evison, J. (2012). A corpus linguistic analysis of turn-openings in spoken academic discourse: Understanding discursive specialisation. English Profile Journal, 3(4), 1–24.
Fraser, B. (1990). An approach to discourse markers. Journal of Pragmatics, 14(3), 383–395.
Gisladottir, R. S., Bögels, S., & Levinson, S. C. (2018). Oscillatory brain responses reflect anticipation during comprehension of speech acts in spoken dialog. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 121, 1–13.
Gumperz, J. J. (1992). Contextualization and understanding. In A. Duranti & C. Goodwin (Eds.), Rethinking Context: Language as an Interactive Phenomenon (pp. 229–252). Cambridge University Press.
Gumperz, J. J. (1996). The linguistic and cultural relativity of inference. In J. J. Gumperz & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Rethinking Linguistic Relativity. Cambridge University Press.
Halliday, M. A. K., & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. Longman.
Heldner, M., & Edlund, J. (2010). Pauses, gaps and overlaps in conversations. Journal of Phonetics, 38(4), 555–568.
Heritage, J. (1998). Oh-prefaced Responses to Inquiry. Language in Society, 27(3), 291–334.
Heritage, J. (2013). Turn-initial position and some of its occupants. Journal of Pragmatics, 571, 331–337.
Heritage, J. (2015). Well-prefaced turns in English conversation: A conversation analytic perspective. Journal of Pragmatics, 881, 88–104.
Heritage, J., & Sorjonen, M. L. (2018). Analyzing turn-initial particles. In J. Heritage & M.-L. Sorjonen (Eds.), Between Turn and Sequence: Turn-initial Particles across Languages (pp. 1–22). John Benjamins.
Hoey, M. (2005). Lexical Priming: A New Theory of Words and Language. Routledge.
Hoffmann, S., Evert, S., Smith, N., Lee, D., & Berglund Prytz, Y. (2008). Corpus Linguistics with BNCweb: A Practical Guide. Peter Lang.
Holler, J., Kendrick, K. H., & Levinson, S. C. (2017). Processing language in face-to-face conversation: Questions with gestures get faster responses. Psychonometric Bulletin Review, 251, 1900–1918.
Indefrey, P., & Levelt, W. J. M. (2004). The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition, 92(1–2), 101–144.
Jefferson, G. (1973). A case of precision timing in ordinary conversation: Overlapped tag-positioned address terms in closing sequences. Semiotics, 9(1), 47–96.
Jefferson, G. (1986). Notes on ‘latency’ in overlap onset. Human Studies, 91, 153–183.
Jescheniak, J. D., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1994). Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4), 824–843.
Jongman, S. R., Piai, V., & Meyer, A. S. (2019). Planning for language production: The electrophysiological signature of attention to the cue to speak. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience. Advance online publication.
Jucker, A. H. (1993). The discourse marker well: A relevance-theoretical account. Journal of Pragmatics, 19(5), 435–452.
De Klerk, V. (2005). Procedural meanings of well in a corpus of Xhosa English. Journal of Pragmatics, 37(8), 1183–1205.
Koivisto, A. (2012). Discourse patterns for turn-final conjunctions. Journal of Pragmatics, 44(10), 1254–1272.
Levelt, W. J. M., Praamstra, P., Meyer, A. S., Helenius, P., & Salmelin, R. (1998). An MEG study of picture naming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(5), 553–567.
Levelt, W. J., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(1), 1–38.
Levinson, S. C. (2003). Contextualizing ‘contextualization cues’. In S. Eerdmans, C. Prevignano, & P. Thibault (Eds.), Language and Interaction: Discussions with John J. Gumperz (pp. 31–39). John Benjamins.
Levinson, S. C. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge University Press.
Levinson, S. C. (2013). Action formation and ascription. In J. Sidnell & T. Stivers (Eds.), The Handbook of Conversation Analysis (pp. 103–130). Wiley Blackwell.
Levinson, S. C. (2016). Turn-taking in human communication: Origins and implications for language processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(1), 6–14.
Levinson, S. C., & Holler, J. (2014). The origin of human multi-modal communication. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369(1651), 20130302.
Levinson, S. C., & Torreira, F. (2015). Timing in turn-taking and its implications for processing models of language. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(731), 1–17.
Magyari, L., Bastiaansen, M. C. M., de Ruiter, J. P., & Levinson, S. C. (2014). Early anticipation lies behind the speed of response in conversation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(11), 2530–2539.
Mehl, M. R., Vazire, S., Ramirez-Esparza, N., Slatcher, R. B., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2007). Are women really more talkative than men?Science, 317(5834), 82.
Norrick, N. (2009). Interjections as pragmatic markers. Journal of Pragmatics, 41(5), 866–891.
Ochs, E., Schegloff, E. A., & Thompson, S. A. (Eds.). (1996). Interaction and Grammar. Cambridge University Press.
Oldfield, R. C., & Wingfield, A. (1965). Response latencies in naming objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 171, 273–28.
Pfeiffer, M. (2016). The deictic dimension of exclamations: On the use of wh-exclamatives in German face-to-face interaction. Revue de Sémantique et Pragmatique, 401, 35–57.
Pomerantz, A., & Heritage, J. (2013). Preference. In J. Sidnell & T. Stivers (Eds.), The Handbook of Conversation Analysis (pp. 210–228). Wiley Blackwell.
Roberts, S. G., Torreira, F., & Levinson, S. C. (2015). The effects of processing and sequence organization on the timing of turn taking: a corpus study. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(509), 1–16.
Rochmont, M., & Cullicover, P. (1990). English Focus Constructions and the Theory of Grammar. Cambridge University Press.
Romero-Trillo, J. (2018). Prosodic modeling and position analysis of pragmatic markers in English conversation. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 141, 169–195.
Rühlemann, C. (2013). Narrative in English Conversation: A Corpus Analysis. Cambridge University Press.
Rühlemann, C. (2018a). Corpus Linguistics for Pragmatics. Routledge.
Rühlemann, C. (2018b). How long does it take to say ‘well’? Evidence from the Audio BNC. Corpus Pragmatics, 3(1), 49–66.
Rühlemann, C., & Gee, M. (2017). Conversation Analysis and the XML method. Gesprächsforschung, 181, 274–296.
Rühlemann, C., & Schweinberger, M. (under review). Information in turns-at-talk: Modeling the placement of nuclear stress.
Sacks, H. (1987). On the preference for agreement and contiguity in sequences in conversation. In G. Button & J. R. E. Lee (Eds.), Talk and Social Organisation. Multilingual Matters.
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organisation of turn-taking for conversation. Language, 50(4), 696–735.
Schegloff, E. A. (1996). Turn organization: One intersection of grammar and interaction. In E. Ochs, E. A. Schegloff, & S. A. Thompson (Eds.), Interaction and Grammar (pp. 52–133). Cambridge University Press.
Schegloff, E. A. (2000). Overlapping talk and the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language in Society, 29(1), 1-63.
Schegloff, E. A., & Lerner, G. H. (2009). Beginning to respond: Well-prefaced responses to wh-questions. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 42(2), 91–115.
Schiffrin, D. (1985). Conversational coherence: The role of well. Language, 61(3), 640–667.
Schiffrin, D. (1987). Discourse Markers. Cambridge University Press.
Schleppegrell, M. (1991). Paratactic because. Journal of Pragmatics, 16(4), 323–337.
Sicoli, M., Stivers, T., Enfield, N. J., & Levinson, S. C. (2015). Marked initial pitch in questions signals marked communicative function. Language and Speech, 581, 204–223.
Stivers, T., Enfield, N. J., Brown, P., Englert, C., Hayashi, M., Heinemann, T., Hoymann, G., Rossano, F., de Ruiter, J. P., Yoon, K.-E., & Levinson, S. C. (2009). Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation. Proceedings of the National Academy of the Sciences. U.S.A. 106(26), 10587–10592.
Stubbs, M. (2002). Words and Phrases: Corpus Studies of Lexical Semantics. Blackwell.
Svartvik, J. (1980). Well in conversation. In S. Greenbaum, G. Leech, & J. Svartvik (Eds.), Studies in English Linguistics: For Randolph Quirk (pp. 167–177). Longman.
Tao, H. (2003). Turn initiators in spoken English: A corpus-based approach to interaction and grammar. In P. Leistyna & C. F. Meyer (Eds.), Corpus Analysis: Language Structure and Language Use (pp. 187–207). Rodopi.
Torreira, F., Bögels, S. & Levinson, S. C. (2015). Breathing for answering: The time course of response planning in conversation. Frontiers in Psychology, 61, 1–11.
Tottie, G. (2015). Turn management and the fillers uh and um. In K. Aijmer & C. Rühlemann (Eds.), Corpus Pragmatics: A Handbook (pp. 381–407). Cambridge University Press.
Vaughan, E., McCarthy, M., & Clancy, B. (2017). Vague category markers as turn-final items in Irish English. World Englishes, 36(2), 208–223.
Yuan, J., M. Liberman, & Cieri, C. (2006). Towards an integrated understanding of speaking rate in conversation. In Interspeech 2006 – ICSLP (pp. 541–544). Pittsburgh, PA. [URL].
Cited by (4)
Cited by four other publications
Dingemanse, Mark
2024. Interjections at the Heart of Language. Annual Review of Linguistics 10:1 ► pp. 257 ff.
Rühlemann, Christoph & Mathias Barthel
2024. Word frequency and cognitive effort in turns-at-talk: turn structure affects processing load in natural conversation. Frontiers in Psychology 15
Robinson, Jeffrey D., Christoph Rühlemann & Daniel Taylor Rodriguez
2022. The Bias Toward Single-Unit Turns in Conversation. Research on Language and Social Interaction 55:2 ► pp. 165 ff.
Rühlemann, Christoph & Martin Schweinberger
2021. Which word gets the nuclear stress in a turn-at-talk?. Journal of Pragmatics 178 ► pp. 426 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 august 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.