Article published In:
International Journal of Corpus Linguistics
Vol. 28:3 (2023) ► pp.291317
References (56)
References
Allan, K., & Burridge, K. (2006). Forbidden Words: Taboo and the Censoring of Language. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arshamian, A., Gerkin, R. C., Kruspe, N., Wnuk, E., Floyd, S., O’Meara, C., Rodriguez, G. G., Lundström, J. N., Mainland, J. D., & Majid, A. (2022). The perception of odor pleasantness is shared across cultures. Current Biology, 32 (9), 2061–2066. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berlin, B., & Kay, P. (1969). Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution. University of California Press.Google Scholar
Boisson, C. P. (1997). La dénomination des odeurs: Variations et régularités linguistiques [The naming of odours: Linguistic variations and regularities]. Intellectica, 24 (1), 29–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41 (4), 977–990. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chastrette, M. (2002). Classification and structure-odor relationships. In C. Rouby, B. Schaal, D. Dubois, R. Gervais, & A. Holley (Eds.), Olfaction, Taste, and Cognition (pp. 100–116). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chrea, C., Ferdenzi, C., Valentin, D., & Abdi, H. (2007). Revisiting the relation between language and cognition: A cross-cultural study with odors. Current Psychology Letters, 22 (2), 1–14.Google Scholar
Chrea, C., Valentin, D., Sulmont-Rossé, C., Ly Mai, H., Hoang Nguyen, D., & Abdi, H. (2004). Culture and odor categorization: Agreement between cultures depends upon the odors. Food Quality and Preference, 15 (7–8), 669–679. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Classen, C., Howes, D., & Synnott, A. (1994). Aroma: The Cultural History of Smell. Routledge.Google Scholar
Croijmans, I., & Majid, A. (2016). Not all flavor expertise is equal: The language of wine and coffee experts. PLoS ONE, 11 (6), 1–21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. (1889). The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. D. Appleton and Company. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davies, M. (2008–). The Corpus of Contemporary American English (COCA): One billion words, 1990–2019. [URL]
Dubois, D. (2000). Categories as acts of meaning: The case of categories in olfaction and audition. Cognitive Science Quarterly, 1 1, 35–68.Google Scholar
Engen, T. (1983). The human uses of olfaction. American Journal of Otolaryngology, 4 (4), 250–251. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Floyd, S., San Roque, L., & Majid, A. (2018). Smell is coded in grammar and frequent in discourse: Cha’palaa olfactory language in cross-linguistic perspective. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 28 (2), 175–196. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibson, E., Futrell, R., Jara-Ettinger, J., Mahowald, K., Bergen, L., Ratnasingam, S., Gibson, M., Piantadosi, S. T., & Conway, B. R. (2017). Color naming across languages reflects color use. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114 (40), 10785–10790. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hörberg, T., Larsson, M., & Olofsson, J. K. (2020). Mapping the semantic organization of the English odor vocabulary using natural language data. PsyArXiv. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Howes, D. (1987). Olfaction and transition: An essay on the ritual uses of smell. Canadian Review of Sociology, 24 (3), 398–416. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iatropoulos, G., Herman, P., Lansner, A., Karlgren, J., Larsson, M., & Olofsson, J. K. (2018). The language of smell: Connecting linguistic and psychophysical properties of odor descriptors. Cognition, 178 1, 37–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iravani, B., Schaefer, M., Wilson, D. A., Arshamian, A., & Lundström, J. N. (2021). The human olfactory bulb process odor valence representation and initiate motor avoidance behavior. bioRxiv. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaeppler, K., & Mueller, F. (2013). Odor classification: A review of factors influencing perception-based odor arrangements. Chemical Senses, 38 1, 189–209. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kemp, C., Xu, Y., & Regier, T. (2018). Semantic typology and efficient communication. Annual Review of Linguistics, 41, 109–128. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kövecses, Z. (2019). Perception and metaphor: The case of smell. In L. J. Speed, C. O’Meara, L. San Roque, & A. Majid (Eds.), Perception Metaphors (pp. 327–346). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krifka, M. (2010). A note on the asymmetry in the hedonic implicatures of olfactory and gustatory terms. In S. Fuchs, P. Hoole, C. Mooshammer, & M. Zygis (Eds.), Between the Regular and the Particular in Speech and Language (pp. 235–245). Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Lee, A. P. (2015). Lexical categories and conceptualization of olfaction in Amis. Language and Cognition, 7 (3), 321–350. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C., & Majid, A. (2014). Differential ineffability and the senses. Mind and Language, 29 (4), 407–427. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Majid, A. (2021). Human olfaction at the intersection of language, culture, and biology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25 (2), 111–123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Majid, A., & Burenhult, N. (2014). Odors are expressible in language, as long as you speak the right language. Cognition, 130 (2), 266–270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Majid, A., Burenhult, N., Stensmyr, M., De Valk, J., & Hansson, B. S. (2018). Olfactory language and abstraction across cultures. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373 (1752). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Majid, A., & Kruspe, N. (2018). Hunter-gatherer olfaction is special. Current Biology, 28 (3), 409–413.e2. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Majid, A., Roberts, S. G., Cilissen, L., Emmorey, K., Nicodemus, B., O’Grady, L., Woll, B., LeLan, B., de Sousa, H., Cansler, B. L., Shayan, S., de Vos, C., Senft, G., Enfield, N. J., Razak, R. A., Fedden, S., Tufvesson, S., Dingemanse, M., Ozturk, O., … Levinson, S. C. (2018). Differential coding of perception in the world’s languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A, 115 (45), 11369–11376. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McGann, J. P. (2017). Poor human olfaction is a 19th-century myth. Science, 356 (6338), eaam7263. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Meara, C., & Majid, A. (2016). How changing lifestyles impact seri smellscapes and smell language. Anthropological Linguistics, 58 (2), 107–131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peeters, G., & Czapinski, J. (1990). Positive-negative asymmetry in evaluations: The distinction between affective and informational negativity effects. European Review of Social Psychology, 1 (1), 33–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Plümacher, M. (2007). Color perception, color description and metaphor. In M. Plümacher & P. Holz (Eds.), Speaking of Colors and Odors (pp. 61–84). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Poulton, T. (2020a). Odours and Odorants (COCA) [Data set]. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020b). The smells we know and love: Variation in codability and description strategy. Language and Cognition, 12 (3), 501–525. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Poulton, T., & Hill, C. (forthcoming). Linguistic descriptions and cultural models of olfaction in Umpila and English. Language Sciences. DOI logo
Rayson, P., Archer, D. E., Piao, S., & McEnery, T. (2004). The UCREL semantic analysis system. Proceedings of the Workshop on Beyond Named Entity Recognition Semantic Labelling for NLP tTasks in Association with LREC 2004. Lisbon (pp. 7–12). [URL]
Rouby, C., & Bensafi, M. (2002). Is there a hedonic dimension to odors? In C. Rouby, B. Schaal, D. Dubois, R. Gervais, & A. Holley (Eds.), Olfaction, Taste, and Cognition (pp. 140–159). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
San Roque, L., Kendrick, K. H., Norcliffe, E., Brown, P., Defina, R., Dingemanse, M., Dirksmeyer, T., Enfield, N. J., Floyd, S., Hammond, J., Rossi, G., Tufvesson, S., van Putten, S., & Majid, A. (2015). Vision verbs dominate in conversation across cultures, but the ranking of non-visual verbs varies. Cognitive Linguistics, 26 (1), 31–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, N., & Lee, S. W. S. (2019). The smell of suspicion: How the nose curbs gullibility. In J. P. Forgas & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), The Social Psychology of Gullibility (pp. 234–252). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shepherd, G. M. (2004). The human sense of smell: Are we better than we think? PLoS Biology, 2 (5), e146. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. (1975). Rethinking Symbolism. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sweetser, E. (1990). From Etymology to Pragmatics: Metaphorical and Cultural Aspects of Semantic Structure. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Valenzuela, J., & Soriano, C. (2008). Sensorial perception as a source domain: A cross-linguistic study [Paper presentation]. VII International Conference on Researching and Applying Metaphor (RaAM 7), Cáceres, Spain.
Vanek, N., Sóskuthy, M., & Majid, A. (2021). Consistent verbal labels promote odor category learning. Cognition, 206 1, 104485. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Viberg, Å. (1983). The verbs of perception: A typological study. In B. Butterworth, B. Comrie, & D. Östen (Eds.), Explanations for Language Universals. Walter de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Warriner, A. B., Kuperman, V., & Brysbaert, M. (2013). Norms of valence, arousal, and dominance for 13,915 English lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 45 (4), 1191–1207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, D. A., & Stevenson, R. J. (2006). Learning to Smell: Olfactory Perception from Neurobiology to Behavior. Johns Hopkins University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Winter, B. (2016). Taste and smell words form an affectively loaded and emotionally flexible part of the English lexicon. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31 (8), 975–988. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019). Sensory Linguistics: Language, Perception and Metaphor. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Winter, B., Perlman, M., & Majid, A. (2018). Vision dominates in perceptual language: English sensory vocabulary is optimized for usage. Cognition, 179 1, 213–220. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wnuk, E., & Majid, A. (2014). Revisiting the limits of language: The odor lexicon of Maniq. Cognition, 131 (1), 125–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yeshurun, Y., & Sobel, N. (2010). An odor is not worth a thousand words: From multidimensional odors to unidimensional odor objects. Annual Review of Psychology, 61 (1), 219–241. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zaslavsky, N., Kemp, C., Tishby, N., & Regier, T. (2019). Color naming reflects both perceptual structure and communicative need. Topics in Cognitive Science, 11 (1), 207–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (1)

Cited by one other publication

Schönefeld, Doris Eveline
2024. Expressing smells in (American) English. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 11 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.