Languages that lack grammatical gender often still index the sex of humans and higher animates through lexical means (Braun 2001). In the Papuan language Nungon, natural sex is indicated lexically, with gendered person and kin terms. Certain person terms may also function as nominal modifiers. Indexation of sex in these person and kin terms is partially dependent on age. The older the speaker or focal person for the kin relationship, the more likely that his/her sex will determine the term chosen to refer to the addressee or secondary person in the kin relationship. Most kin and person terms for small children disregard the sex of the child; such terms instead employ the sex of the focal person to describe the relationship with the child. Unlike with children, there are no completely gender-neutral terms for adults, although the dedicated male person terms amna, “man” and ketket, “boy” function in certain contexts with generic reference, meaning “human” and “youth.” Generic application of amna, “man” relates to syntax: amna as object argument of deverbal participle expressions has generic reference, as does amna under negation. Thus, indexation of sex is seen to be partially dependent (per Aikhenvald & Dixon 1998) on negation.
Aikhenvald, A. (2010). Imperatives and commands. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Aikhenvald, A., & Dixon, R.M.W. (1998). Dependencies in grammatical systems. Language, 741, 56–80.
Braun, F. (2001). The communication of gender in Turkish. In M. Hellinger & H. Bussmann (Eds.), Gender across languages, Vol. 11 (pp. 283–310). Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Engelberg, M. (2001). The communication of gender in Finnish. In M. Hellinger & H. Bussmann (Eds.), Gender across languages, Vol. 21 (pp. 109–132). Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Fleming, L., & Slotta, J. (2015). Social pragmatics and types of person reference: The case of proper names vs. kin terms. Presented at the Chicago Linguistic Society 51 Meeting. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.
Matthiessen, P. (1962). Under the mountain wall: A chronicle of two seasons in the stone age. New York: Vintage.
Haas, M. (1944). Men’s and women’s speech in Koasati. Language, 201, 142–149.
McElhanon, K. (1967). Preliminary observations on the Huon Peninsula languages. Oceanic Linguistics, 1(1), 1–45.
Sarvasy, H. (2014). A Grammar of Nungon, a Papuan language of Morobe Province, Papua New Guinea. PhD Dissertation. Cairns: James Cook University.
Sarvasy, H. (2015a). Split number in Nungon. Presented at the Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America. Portland: Linguistic Society of America.
Sarvasy, H. (2015b). Imperatives and commands in Nungon. Presented at Workshop on Commands. Cairns: James Cook University.
Wegmann, U. (1994). Anthropology background study. Manuscript. Ukarumpa: Summer Institute of Linguistics.
Wassmann, J. & Dasen, P.R. (1994). “Hot” and “Cold”: Classification and sorting among the Yupno of Papua New Guinea. International Journal of Psychology, 29(1), 19–38.
2019. The root nominal stage: a case study of early Nungon verbs. Journal of Child Language 46:6 ► pp. 1073 ff.
Sarvasy, Hannah S.
2020. The Acquisition of Clause Chaining in Nungon. Frontiers in Psychology 11
Sarvasy, Hannah S.
2021. Acquisition of multi-verb predicates in Nungon. First Language 41:4 ► pp. 478 ff.
Sarvasy, Hannah
2017. Quantification in Nungon. In Handbook of Quantifiers in Natural Language: Volume II [Studies in Linguistics and Philosophy, 97], ► pp. 609 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 august 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.