Review published In:
Interpreting
Vol. 1:2 (1996) ► pp.267270
References
Casado, B., & Jiménez, I.
(1996) Mental processes involved in conference interpretation: Verbal fluency. Unpublished Master’s thesis, University of Geneva.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D.
(1993) Working memory and language. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gordon, S. E.
(1992) Implications of cognitive theory for knowledge acquisition. In R. R. Hoffman (Ed.), The psychology of expertise. Cognitive research and empirical AI (pp. 99–120). New York: Springer Verlag.Google Scholar
Grassi, S., & Mazzoleni, D.
(1996) Les tests d’aptitude pour I’interprétation de conférence: Le Delayed Auditory Feedback (DAF). Unpublished Master’s thesis, University of Geneva.Google Scholar
Künzli, A., & Moser-Mercer, B.
(1995) Human strategies for translation and interpretation. In L. Dreschler-Fischer & S. Pribbenow (Eds.), KI-Activities: Workshops, posters, demos. Extended abstracts (pp. 304–306). Bonn: Gesellschaft für Informatik.Google Scholar
Seamster, T. L., Redding, R. E., Cannon, J. R., Ryder, J. M., & Purcell, J. A.
(1993) Cognitive task analysis of expertise in air traffic control. The International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 3:4, 257–283. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Frensch, P. A.
(1992) On being an expert: A cost-benefit analysis. In R. R. Hoffman (Ed.), The psychology of expertise. Cognitive research and empirical AI (pp. 191–203). New York: Springer Verlag.Google Scholar