Article published in:
Interpreting
Vol. 16:2 (2014) ► pp. 139168
References

References

Adobe Systems Incorporated
(2003) Adobe Audition (Version 1.0) [Computer software] Available from www​.adobe​.com
Andrews, G., Birney, D. & Halford, G.S.
(2006) Relational processing and working memory capacity in comprehension of relative clause sentences. Memory & Cognition 34 (6), 1325–1340. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Anthony, L.
(2011) AntConc (Version 3.2.4) [Computer Software]>. Tokyo, Japan: Waseda University. Available from http://​www​.antlab​.sci​.waseda​.ac​.jp
Baddeley, A.
(1996) Exploring the central executive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 49A (1), 5–28. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2000) The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4 (11), 417–423. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A.D.
(2002) Is working memory still working? European Psychologist 7 (2), 85–97. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S. & Papagno, C.
(1998) The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review 105 (1), 158–173. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A.D. & Hitch, G.J.
(1974) Working memory. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory. Vol. 8. New York: Academic Press, 47–89. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bajo, M.T., Padilla, F. & Padilla, P.
(2000) Comprehension processes in simultaneous interpreting. In A. Chesterman, N. Gallardo San Salvador & Y. Gambier (Eds.), Translation in context. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 127–142. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bülow-Møller, A.M.
(1999) Existential problems. On the processing of irrealis in simultaneous interpreting. Interpreting 4 (2), 145–168. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Caplan, D. & Waters, G.S.
(1999) Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22, 77–126. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, P.A., Just, M.A. & Reichle, E.D.
(2000) Working memory and executive function: Evidence from neuroimaging. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 10, 195–199. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cattell, R.B. & Cattell, A.K.S.
(1950) Test of g: Culture free. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.Google Scholar
Christoffels, I.
(2004) Cognitive studies in simultaneous interpreting. Ipskamp/Enschede: PrintPartners.Google Scholar
Clark, H.H.
(1969) Linguistic processes in deductive reasoning. Psychological Review 76, 387–404. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Conway, A.R.A., Cowan, N., Bunting, M.F., Therriault, D.J. & Minkoff, S.R.B.
(2002) A latent variable analysis of working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, processing speed, and general fluid intelligence. Intelligence 30, 163–183. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Conway, A.R.A., Kane, M.J., Bunting, M.F., Hambrick, D.Z., Wilhelm, O. & Engle, R.W.
(2005) Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and a user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 12 (5), 769–786. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cowan, N.
(1988) Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information-processing system. Psychological Bulletin 104 (2), 163–191. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1999) An embedded-processes model of working memory. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 62–101. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2000) Processing limits of selective attention and working memory: Potential implications for interpreting. Interpreting 5 (2), 117–146. Doi: CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M. & Carpenter, P.A.
(1980) Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 19, 450–466. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1983) Individual differences in integrating information between and within sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 9 (4), 561–584. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M. & Green, I.
(1986) Individual differences in comprehending and producing words in context. Journal of Memory and Language 25, 1–18. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Darò, V.
(1989) The role of memory and attention in simultaneous interpretation: A neurolinguistic approach. The Interpreters’ Newsletter 2, 50–56.Google Scholar
de Groot, A.M.B.
(1997) The cognitive study of translation and interpretation: Three approaches. In J.H. Danks, G.M. Shreve, S.B. Fountain & M.K. McBeath (Eds.), Cognitive processes in translation and interpretation. Thousand Oaks/London/ New Delhi: Sage, 25–56.Google Scholar
Engle, R.W.
(2002) Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science 11 (1), 19–23. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Engle, R.W. & Conway, A.R.A.
(1998) Working memory and comprehension. In R.H. Logie & K.J. Gilhooly (Eds.), Working memory and thinking. Hove: Psychology Press, 67–91.Google Scholar
Engle, R.W. & Kane, M.J.
(2004) Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control. In B. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory. Vol. 44. New York: Academic Press, 145–199.Google Scholar
Engle, R.W., Tuholski, S.W., Laughlin, J.E. & Conway, A.R.A.
(1999) Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 128 (3), 309–331. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K.A. & Delaney, P.F.
(1998) Working memory and expert performance. In R.H. Logie & K.J. Gilhooly (Eds.), Working memory and thinking. Hove: Psychology Press, 93–114.Google Scholar
Fabbro, F. & Darò, V.
(1995) Delayed auditory feedback in polyglot simultaneous interpreters. Brain and Language 48, 309–319. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fan, J., McCandliss, B.D., Sommer, T., Raz, A. & Posner, M.I.
(2002) Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14 (3), 340–347. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G. & Buchner, A.
(2007) G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods 39, 175–191. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Feldman Barrett, L., Tugade, M.M. & Engle, R.W.
(2004) Individual differences in working memory capacity and dual-process theories of the mind. Psychological Bulletin 130 (4), 553–573. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, N.P. & Miyake, A.
(2004) The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 133 (1), 101–135. Doi: CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S.E. & Baddeley, A.D.
(1993) Phonological working memory – a critical building-block for reading development and vocabulary acquisition. European Journal of Psychology of Education 8 (3), 259–272. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gernsbacher, M.A. & Foertsch, J.A.
(1999) Three models of discourse comprehension. In S. Garrod & M.J. Pickering (Eds.), Language processing. Hove: Psychology Press, 283–299.Google Scholar
Gerver, D.
(1969/2002). The effects of source language presentation rate on the performance of simultaneous conference interpreters. In F. Pöchhacker & M. Shlesinger (Eds.), The interpreting studies reader. London/New York: Routledge, 52–66.Google Scholar
(1974) The effects of noise on the performance of simultaneous interpreters: Accuracy of performance. Acta Psychologica 38, 159–167. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gile, D.
(1995) Basic concepts and models for interpreter and translator training. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goldman-Eisler, F.
(1972) Segmentation of input in simultaneous translation. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 1 (2), 127–140. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hester, R. & Garavan, H.
(2005) Working memory and executive function: The influence of content and load on the control of attention. Memory and Cognition 33 (2), 221–233. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hodáková, S.
(2009) Pamäť v simultánnom a konzekutívnom tlmočení. [Memory in simultaneous and consecutive interpreting.] Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation. Nitra,Konštantín Filozof University.Google Scholar
Iman, R.L. & Conover, W.J.
(1979) The use of the rank transform in regression. Technometrics 21 (4), 499–509. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jarrold, C. & Towse, J.N.
(2006) Individual differences in working memory. Neuroscience 139, 39–50. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Just, M.A. & Carpenter, P.A.
(1992) A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review 99 (1), 122–149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
King, J. & Just, M.A.
(1991) Individual differences in syntactic processing: The role of working memory. Journal of Memory and Language 30, 580–602. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Köpke, B. & Nespoulous, J.-L.
(2006) Working memory performance in expert and novice interpreters. Interpreting 8 (1), 1–23. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Köpke, B. & Signorelli, T.M.
(2012) Methodological aspects of working memory assessment in simultaneous interpreters. International Journal of Bilingualism 16 (2), 183–197. Doi: CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lamberger-Felber, H.
(2001) Text-oriented research into interpreting. Examples from a case-study. Hermes 26, 39–63.Google Scholar
Lee, T.-H.
(2002) Ear-voice span in English into Korean simultaneous interpretation. Meta 47 (4), 596–606. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liu, M., Schallert, D.L. & Carroll, P.J.
(2004) Working memory and expertise in simultaneous interpreting. Interpreting 6 (1), 19–42. Doi: CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Logie, R.H.
(2006) Testing the world’s working memory. Invited talk at the University of Ghent, Ghent, Belgium, 20 November 2006.
Miyake, A., Just, M.A. & Carpenter, P.A.
(1994) Working memory constraints on the resolution of lexical ambiguity: Maintaining multiple interpretations in neural contexts. Journal of Memory and Language 33 (2), 175–202. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N.P., Emerson, M.J., Witzki, A.H., Howerter, A. & Wager, T.D.
(2000) The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 41, 49–100. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mizuno, A.
(2005) Process model for simultaneous interpreting and working memory. Meta 50 (2), 739–752. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Moser-Mercer, B.
(1997) The expert-novice paradigm in interpreting research. In E. Fleischmann, W. Kutz & P.A. Schmitt (Eds.), Translationsdidaktik. Grundfragen der Übersetzungswissenschaft. Tübingen: Gunter Narr, 255–261.Google Scholar
(2005) Simultaneous interpreting and cognitive limitations: The acquisition of expertise as a process of circumventing constraints. Keynote speech delivered at the Workshop on “Cognitive aspects of simultaneous interpreting”, 19-20 May 2005, Université de Toulouse-Le Mirail.
Moser-Mercer, B., Frauenfelder, U.H., Casado, B. & Künzli, A.
(2000) Searching to define expertise in interpreting. In B. Englund Dimitrova & K. Hyltenstam (Eds.), Language processing and simultaneous interpreting. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 107–131. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Oberauer, K., Süss, H.-M., Wilhelm, O. & Wittman, W.W.
(2003) The multiple faces of working memory: Storage, processing, supervision and coordination. Intelligence 31, 167–193. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Petite, C.
(2003) Repairs in simultaneous interpreting: Quality improvement or simple error correction? In A. Collados Aís, M.M. Fernández Sánchez & D. Gile (Eds.), La evaluación de la calidad en interpretación: Investigación [Quality assessment in interpreting: Research].Granada: Comares, 61–71.Google Scholar
Rogers, R.D. & Monsell, S.
(1995) Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 124 (2), 207–231. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, W., Eschman, A. & Zuccolotto, A.
(2002) E-Prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc.Google Scholar
Setton, R.
(1999) Simultaneous interpretation: A cognitive-pragmatic analysis. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stroop, J.R.
(1935) Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 18, 643–662. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Swets, B., Desmet, T., Hambrick D.Z. & Ferreira, F.
(2007) The role of working memory in syntactic ambiguity resolution: A psychometric approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 136 (1), 64–81. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Timarová, Š.
(2008) Working memory and simultaneous interpreting. In P. Boulogne (Ed.), Translation and its others: Selected papers of the CETRA Research Seminar in Translation Studies 2007. Available from http://​www2​.arts​.kuleuven​.be​/info​/bestanden​-div​/Timarova​.pdf.Google Scholar
(2012) Working memory in conference simultaneous interpreting. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Charles University, Prague/KU Leuven.Google Scholar
Timarová, Š., Dragsted, B. & Hansen, I.G.
(2011) Time lag in translation and interpreting. A methodological exploration. In C. Alvstad, A. Hild & E. Tiselius (Eds.), Methods and strategies of process research: Integrative approaches in translation studies. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 121–146. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tzou, Y.-Z., Eslami, Z.R., Chen, H.-C. & Vaid, J.
(2012) Effects of language proficiency and degree of formal training in simultaneous interpreting on working memory and interpreting performance: Evidence from Mandarin-English speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism 16 (2), 213–227. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Was, C.A. & Woltz, D.J.
(2007) Reexamining the relationship between working memory and comprehension: The role of available long-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language 56, 86–102. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Waters, G.S. & Caplan, D.
(1996) The measurement of verbal working memory capacity and its relation to reading comprehension. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 49A (1), 51–79. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Waters, G. & Caplan, D.
(2005) The relationship between age, processing speed, working memory capacity, and language comprehension. Memory 13 (3/4),403–413.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 31 other publications

Abdel Latif, Muhammad M. M.
2018. Towards a typology of pedagogy-oriented translation and interpreting research. The Interpreter and Translator Trainer 12:3  pp. 322 ff. Crossref logo
Chen, Peiyao, Sayuri Hayakawa & Viorica Marian
2020. Cognitive and linguistic predictors of bilingual single-word translation. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science 4:2  pp. 145 ff. Crossref logo
Chen, Sijia
2021. The process and product of note-taking and consecutive interpreting: empirical data from professionals and students. Perspectives  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
CHMIEL, AGNIESZKA, AGNIESZKA SZARKOWSKA, DANIJEL KORŽINEK, AGNIESZKA LIJEWSKA, ŁUKASZ DUTKA, ŁUKASZ BROCKI & KRZYSZTOF MARASEK
2017. Ear–voice span and pauses in intra- and interlingual respeaking: An exploratory study into temporal aspects of the respeaking process. Applied Psycholinguistics 38:5  pp. 1201 ff. Crossref logo
Collard, Camille & Bart Defrancq
2019. Predictors of ear-voice span, a corpus-based study with special reference to sex. Perspectives 27:3  pp. 431 ff. Crossref logo
Diamond, Bruce J. & Gregory M. Shreve
2017.  In The Handbook of Translation and Cognition,  pp. 476 ff. Crossref logo
Dong, Yanping
2018. Complex dynamic systems in students of interpreting training. Translation and Interpreting Studies 13:2  pp. 185 ff. Crossref logo
Dong, Yanping & Ping Li
2020. Attentional control in interpreting: A model of language control and processing control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 23:4  pp. 716 ff. Crossref logo
Dong, Yanping & Fei Zhong
2017. Interpreting experience enhances early attentional processing, conflict monitoring and interference suppression along the time course of processing. Neuropsychologia 95  pp. 193 ff. Crossref logo
Dong, Yanping & Fei Zhong
2019.  In The Handbook of the Neuroscience of Multilingualism,  pp. 685 ff. Crossref logo
Díaz-Galaz, Stephanie & Alejandro Torres
2019. Comprehension in interpreting and translation: testing the phonological interference hypothesis. Perspectives 27:4  pp. 622 ff. Crossref logo
Ferreira, Aline, John W. Schwieter & Julia Festman
2020. Cognitive and Neurocognitive Effects From the Unique Bilingual Experiences of Interpreters. Frontiers in Psychology 11 Crossref logo
Han, Chao
2018. Using rating scales to assess interpretation. Interpreting. International Journal of Research and Practice in Interpreting 20:1  pp. 63 ff. Crossref logo
Hervais-Adelman, Alexis, Barbara Moser-Mercer, Micah M. Murray & Narly Golestani
2017. Cortical thickness increases after simultaneous interpretation training. Neuropsychologia 98  pp. 212 ff. Crossref logo
Ho, Chen-En
2021. What does professional experience have to offer?. Translation, Cognition & Behavior 4:1  pp. 47 ff. Crossref logo
Hyeyeon Chung
2018. Sequentiality in Interpreters’ Memory. The Journal of Translation Studies 19:1  pp. 227 ff. Crossref logo
Korpal, Paweł & Katarzyna Stachowiak-Szymczak
2018. The whole picture: Processing of numbers and their context in simultaneous interpreting. Poznan Studies in Contemporary Linguistics 54:3  pp. 335 ff. Crossref logo
Mellinger, Christopher D. & Thomas A. Hanson
2019. Meta-analyses of simultaneous interpreting and working memory. Interpreting. International Journal of Research and Practice in Interpreting 21:2  pp. 165 ff. Crossref logo
Nour, Soudabeh, Esli Struys, Evy Woumans, Ily Hollebeke & Hélène Stengers
2020. An interpreter advantage in executive functions?. Interpreting. International Journal of Research and Practice in Interpreting 22:2  pp. 163 ff. Crossref logo
Rosiers, Alexandra, Koen Plevoets & June Eyckmans
2020. Choosing to become an interpreter. Translation, Cognition & Behavior 3:1  pp. 25 ff. Crossref logo
Rosiers, Alexandra, Evy Woumans, Wouter Duyck & June Eyckmans
2019. Investigating the presumed cognitive advantage of aspiring interpreters. Interpreting. International Journal of Research and Practice in Interpreting 21:1  pp. 115 ff. Crossref logo
Schwieter, John W. & Aline Ferreira
2017.  In The Handbook of Translation and Cognition,  pp. 144 ff. Crossref logo
Song, Shuxian & Dechao Li
2020. The Predicting Power of Cognitive Fluency for the Development of Utterance Fluency in Simultaneous Interpreting. Frontiers in Psychology 11 Crossref logo
Stachowiak-Szymczak, Katarzyna & Paweł Korpal
2019. Interpreting Accuracy and Visual Processing of Numbers in Professional and Student Interpreters: an Eye-tracking Study. Across Languages and Cultures 20:2  pp. 235 ff. Crossref logo
Su, Wenchao
2020.  In Eye-Tracking Processes and Styles in Sight Translation [New Frontiers in Translation Studies, ],  pp. 19 ff. Crossref logo
Su, Wenchao & Defeng Li
2021. Exploring the effect of interpreting training: Eye-tracking English-Chinese sight interpreting. Lingua 256  pp. 103094 ff. Crossref logo
SUI, LONGJIAO, HAIDEE KRUGER & HELEN SLATYER
2019. The central processing bottleneck during word production: Comparing simultaneous interpreters, bilinguals and monolinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 22:5  pp. 968 ff. Crossref logo
Van der Linden, Lize, Eowyn Van de Putte, Evy Woumans, Wouter Duyck & Arnaud Szmalec
2018. Does Extreme Language Control Training Improve Cognitive Control? A Comparison of Professional Interpreters, L2 Teachers and Monolinguals. Frontiers in Psychology 9 Crossref logo
Wang, Jihong
2016. The relationship between working memory capacity and simultaneous interpreting performance. Interpreting. International Journal of Research and Practice in Interpreting 18:1  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
胡, 敏霞
2020. The Impact of Cognitive Control on the Interpreting Proficiency of Trainee Interpreters. Modern Linguistics 08:05  pp. 647 ff. Crossref logo
胡, 敏霞
2021. Review and Commentary on the Interactive Effects of Interpreting Experience and Executive Control. Modern Linguistics 09:02  pp. 283 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 29 august 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.