Review published In:
Interaction Studies
Vol. 15:1 (2014) ► pp.129142
References
Attenborough, D.
(2009) Monkey lying: Capuchins sounding false alarms. Available: [URL]. (Broadcast in BBC Clever Monkeys documentary. Visited 15 August 2013).
Call, J., & Tomasello, M.
(2009) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Science, 121, 187–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(2005) Three factors in language design. Linguistic Inquiry, 36(1). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) Some simple evo-devo theses: How true would they be for language? In R. Larson, H. Yamakido, & V. Deprez (Eds.), Evolution of human language: Biolinguistic perspectives. CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A.
(2011) The brain basis of language processing: From structure to function. Physiological Reviews, 91(4), 1357–1392. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A., Mueller, J., & Oberecker, R.
(2011) Precursors to natural grammar learning: Preliminary evidence from 4-month-old infants. PLoS One, 6(3). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Graf Estes, K., Evans, J., Alibali, M., & Saffran, J.
(2007) Can infants map meaning to newly segmented words? Statistical segmentation and word learning. Psychological Science, 181, 254–260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, T.
(2002) The faculty of language: What is it, who has it and how did it evolve? Science, 2981, 1569–1579. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M. D., Weiss, D., & Marcus, G.
(2002) Rule learning by cotton-top tamarins. Cognition, 86(1), B15–B22. (retracted) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hickok, G.
(2010) The role of mirror neurons in speech and language processing. Brain and Language, 1121. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D.
(2004) Dorsal and ventral streams: a framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language. Cognition, 921. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kirby, S., & Hurford, J.
(2002) The emergence of linguistic structure: an overview of the iterated learning model. In A. Cangelosi, & D. Parisi (Eds.), Simulating the evolution of language. Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P.
(2004) Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 51, 831–843. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Is speech learning ‘gated’ by the social brain? Developmental Science, 101, 110–120. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lewis, J.
(2009) As well as words: Congo pygmy hunting, mimicry and play. In R. Botha, & C. Knight (Eds.), The cradle of language. OUP.Google Scholar
Lindblom, B., Diehl, R., Park, S. -H., & Salvi, G.
(2011) Sound systems are shaped by their users: the recombination of phonetic substance. In N. Clements, & R. Ridouane (Eds.), Where do phonological features come from? Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lyon, C., Dickerson, B., & Nehaniv, C. L.
Lyon, C., Sato, Y., Saunders, J., & Nehaniv, C. L.
(2009) What is needed for a robot to acquire grammar? Some underlying primitive mechanisms for the synthesis of linguistic ability. IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development, 11, 187–195. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marean, C. W.
(2007) Early human use of marine resources and pigment in South Africa during the Middle Pleistocene. Nature, 4491, 905–908. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) When the sea saved humanity. Scientific American, 221, 52–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meyer, P., Mecklinger, A., Grunwald, T., Fell, J., Elger, C. E., & Friederici, A. D.
(2005) Language processing within the medial temporal lobe. Hippocampus, 151, 451–459. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mielke, J.
(2008) The emergence of distinctive features. OUP.Google Scholar
Oudeyer, P. -Y.
(2006) Self-organization in the evolution of speech. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Poeppel, D., Idsardi, W. J., & van Wassenhove, V.
(2007) Speech perception at the interface of neurobiology and linguistics. Phil. Trans. of the Royal Society, 3631, 1071–1086. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pulvermuller, F., & Fadiga, L.
(2010) Active perception: sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(5), 351–360. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saffran, J., Aslin, R., & Newport, E.
(1996) Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 2741, 1926–1928. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Steels, L.
(1999) The talking heads experiment. Brussels: VUB.Google Scholar
(2001) Language games for autonomous robots. Intelligent Systems, IEEE, 16(5), 16–22.Google Scholar
Wheeler, B. C.
(2009) Monkeys crying wolf? Tufted capuchin monkeys use anti-predator calls to usurp resources from con-specifics. Proc. of the Royal Society, 276(1669), 3013–3018. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Frank, Roslyn M.
2017. Expanding the Scope of Cultural Linguistics: Taking Parrots Seriously. In Advances in Cultural Linguistics [Cultural Linguistics, ],  pp. 529 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 31 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.