Article published In:
Coordination, Collaboration and Cooperation: Interdisciplinary perspectives
Edited by Federica Amici and Lucas M. Bietti
[Interaction Studies 16:3] 2015
► pp. 419450
References (140)
References
Abraham, W.C. (2008). Metaplasticity – tuning synapses and networks for plasticity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 91, 387–399. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Allen, M.L., Haywood, S., Rajendran, T., & Branigan, H.P. (2011). Evidence for syntactic alignment in children with autism. Developmental Science, 141, 540–548. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andersson, J., & Rönnberg, J. (1995). Recall suffers from collaboration: Joint recall effects of friendship and task complexity. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 91, 199–211. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bahrami, B., Olsen, K., Latham, P.E., Roepstorff, A., Rees, G., & Frith, C.D. (2010). Optimally interacting minds. Science, 329(5995), 1081–1085. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bangerter, A. (2000). Identifying individual and collective acts of remembering in task- related communication. Discourse Processes, 301, 237–264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bangerter, A., & Clark, H.H. (2003). Navigating joint projects with dialogue. Cognitive Science, 271, 195–225. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barber, S.J., Rajaram, S., & Aron, A. (2010). When two is too many: Collaborative encoding impairs memory. Memory & Cognition, 381, 255–264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barber, S.J., & Rajaram, S. (2011). Collaborative memory and part-set cueing impairments: The role of executive depletion in modulating retrieval disruption. Memory, 191, 378–397. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barber, S.J., Rajaram, S., & Fox, E.B. (2012). Learning and remembering with others: The key role of retrieval in shaping group recall and collective memory. Social Cognition, 301, 121–132. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J.A., & Chartrand, T.L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 541, 462–479. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barnier, A.J., Sutton, J., Harris, C.B., & Wilson, R.A, (2008). A conceptual and empirical framework for the social distribution of cognition: The case of memory. Cognitive Systems Research, 9(1–2), 33–51. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bartlett, F. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Basden, B.H., Basden, D.R., Bryner, S., & Thomas, R.L., III. (1997). A comparison of group and individual remembering: Does collaboration disrupt retrieval strategies? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 231, 1176–1191. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Basden, B.H., Basden, D.R., & Henry, S. (2001). Costs and benefits of collaborative remembering. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 141, 497–507. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergman, E., & Roediger, H.L. (1999). Can Bartlett’s repeated reproduction experiments be replicated? Memory & Cognition, 271, 937–947. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bernieri, F.J., & Rosenthal, R. (1991). Interpersonal coordination: Behavior matching and interactional synchrony. In R.S. Feldman & B. Rim (Eds.), Fundamentals of nonverbal behaviour: Studies in emotions & social interaction (pp. 401–432). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bernieri, F., Davis, J., Rosenthal, R., & Knee, C. (1994). Interactional synchrony and rapport: Measuring synchrony in displays devoid of sound and facial affect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20(3), 303–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bietti, L.M. (2010). Sharing memories, family conversation and interaction. Discourse & Society, 21(5), 499–523. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2012). Towards a cognitive pragmatics of collective remembering. Pragmatics & Cognition, 20(1), 32–61. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2014). Discursive remembering: Individual and collective remembering as a discursive, cognitive and historical process. Berlin/New York: Walter de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bietti, L.M., & Galiana Castelló, F. (2013). Embodied reminders in family interactions: Multimodal collaboration in remembering activities. Discourse Studies, 15(6), 665–686. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bietti, L.M., Kok, K., & Cienki, A. (2013). Temporal aspects of behavioral alignment in collaborative remembering. In Proceedings of the Tilburg Gesture Research Meeting 2013 . Available at: [URL].
Bietti, L.M., & Baker, M.J. (submitted). Collaborating to remember collaborative creativity: A case study.
Blumen, H., & Rajaram, S. (2008). Effects of group collaboration and repeated retrieval on Individual Recall. Memory, 161, 231–244. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brennan, S.E., Galati, A., & Kuhlen, A. (2010). Two minds, one dialog: Coordinating speaking and understanding. In B. Ross (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation, vol. 531. (pp. 301–345). Academic Press/Elsevier.Google Scholar
Brown, A., Coman, A., & Hirst, W. (2009). Expertise and the formation of collective memory. Social Psychology, 401, 118–128. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carbon, C.C., & Albrecht, S. (2012). Bartlett’s schema theory: The unreplicated “portrait d’homme” series from 1932. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(11), 2258–2270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chartrand, T.L., & Lakin, J. (2013). The antecedents and consequences of human behavioral mimicry. Annual Review of Psychology, 641, 285–308. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cienki, A., Bietti, L.M., & Kok, K. (2014). Multimodal alignment during collaborative remembering. Memory Studies, 7(3), 354–369. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H.H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H.H., & Brennan, S.A. (1991). Grounding in communication. In L.B. Resnick, J.M. Levine, & S.D. Teasley (Eds.), Perspectives on socially shared cognition (pp.127–148). Washington: APA Books. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coman, A., Brown, A.D., Koppel, J., & Hirst, W. (2009). Collective memory from a psychological perspective. International Journal of Politics, Culture and Society, 22(2), 125–141.Google Scholar
Coman, A., Kolling, A., Lewis, M., & Hirst, W. (2012). Mnemonic convergence: From empirical data to large-scale dynamics. In Social Computing, behavioral - cultural modeling and prediction. Lecture Notes in computer science, vol. 72271 (pp. 256–265). Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Condon, W.S., & Ogston, W.D. (1966). Sound film analysis of normal and pathological behavior patterns. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1431, 338–347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Congleton, A.R., & Rajaram, S. (2011). The influence of learning methods on collaboration: Prior repeated retrieval enhances retrieval organization, abolishes collaborative inhibition, and promotes post-collaborative memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1401, 535–551. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cowley, S. (2014). Linguistic embodiment and verbal constraints: Human cognition and the scales of time. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(1085). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cuc, A., Ozuru, Y., Manier, D., & Hirst, W. (2006). The transformation of collective memories: Studies of family recounting. Memory & Cognition, 341, 752–762. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dale, R., Fusaroli, R., Duran, N.D., & Richardson, D.C. (2013). The self- organization of human interaction. In B. Ross (Ed.), Psychology of Learning and motivation (pp. 43–95). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Dijkstra, K., & Zwaan, R.A. (2014). Memory and action. In L. Shapiro (Ed.), The routledge handbook of embodied cognition (pp. 286–305). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
. (1993). Précis of Origins of the modern mind. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 161, 737–791. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2007). The slow process: A hypothetical cognitive adaptation for distributed cognitive networks. Journal of Physiology (Paris), 1011, 214–222. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Echterhoff, G., Groll, S., & Hirst, W. (2007). Tainted truth: Overcorrection for misinformation influence on eyewitness memory. Social Cognition, 251, 367–409. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fagin, M.M., Yamashiro, J.K., & Hirst, W. (2013). The adaptive function of distributed remembering: Contributions to the formation of collective memory. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 4(1), 91–106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fusaroli, R., & Tylén, K. (2012). Carving language for social coordination: A dynamical approach. Interaction Studies, 13(1), 103–124. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fusaroli, R., Bahrami, B., Olsen, K., Roepstorff, A., Rees, G., Frith, C., & Tylén, K. (2012). Coming to Terms: Quantifying the benefits of linguistic coordination. Psychological Science, 23(8), 931–939. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gauld, A., & Stephenson, G.M. (1967). Some experiments related to Bartlett’s theory of remembering. British Journal of Psychology, 581, 39–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Nusbaum, H., Kelly, S., & Wagner, S. (2001). Explaining math: Gesturing lightens the load. Psychological Science, 21, 516–522. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Alibali, M.W (2013). Gestures role in speaking, learning, and creating language. Annual Review of Psychology, 1231, 448–453.Google Scholar
Gordon, B.R., & Theiner, G. (2015). Scaffolded joint action as a micro foundation of organizational learning. In C.B. Stone & L.M. Bietti (Eds.), Contextualizing human memory: An interdisciplinary approach to understanding how individuals and groups remember the past (pp. 154–186). London: Psychology Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harris, C.B., Paterson, H.M., & Kemp, R.I. (2008). Collaborative recall and collective memory: What happens when we remember together? Memory, 161, 213–230. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harris, C.B., Keil, P.G., Sutton, J., Barnier, A., & McIlwain, D. (2011). We remember, we forget: Collaborative remembering in older couples. Discourse Processes, 481, 267–303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harris, C.B., Barnier, A.J., & Sutton, J. (2012). Consensus collaboration enhances group and individual recall accuracy. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 651, 179–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2013). Shared encoding and the costs and benefits of collaborative recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 391, 183–195. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harris, C.B., Barnier, A.J., Sutton, J., & Keil, P.G. (2014). Couples as socially distributed cognitive systems: Remembering in everyday social and material contexts. Memory Studies, 7(3), 285–297. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Henkel, L.A., & Rajaram, S. (2011). Collaborative remembering in older adults: Age-invariant outcomes in the context of episodic memory deficit. Psychology & Aging, 261, 532–545. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hirst, W., & Manier, D. (1996). Social influences on remembering. In D. Rubin (Ed.), Remembering the past (pp. 271–290). New York: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hirst, W., Manier, D., & Apetroaia, I. (1997). The social construction of the remembered self: Family recounting. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 8181, 163–188. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hirst, W., & Manier, D. (2008). Towards a psychology of collective memory. Memory, 16(3), 183–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hirst, W., & Echterhoff, G. (2012). Remembering in conversations: The social sharing and reshaping of memories. Annual Review of Psychology, 631, 55–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Holler, J., & Wilkin, K. (2011). Co-speech gesture mimicry in the process of collaborative referring during face-to-face dialogue. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 351, 133–153. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hollingshead, A.B. (1998). Communication, learning and retrieval in transactive memory systems. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 341, 423–442. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hutchins, E. (2014). The cultural ecosystem of human cognition. Philosophical Psychology, 27(1), 34–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hyman, I.E., Cardwell, B.A., & Roy, R.A. (2013). Multiple causes of collaborative inhibition in memory for categorised word lists. Memory, 21(7), 875–890. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ireland, M.E., Slatcher, R.B., Eastwick, P.W., Scissors, L.E., Finkel, E.J., & Pennebaker, J.W. (2011). Language style matching predicts relationship initiation and stability. Psychological Science, 22(1), 39–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackson, M., & Moreland, R.L. (2009). Transactive memory in the classroom. Small Group Research, 40(5), 508–534. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kashima, Y. (2000). Maintaining cultural stereotypes in the serial reproduction of narratives. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(5), 594–604. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Knoblich, G., Butterfill, S., & Sebanz, N. (2011). Psychological research on joint action: Theory and data. In B. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, vol. 541 (pp. 59–101). Burlington: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lemke, J.L. (2000). Across the scales of time: Artifacts, activities and meanings in ecosocial systems. Mind, Culture and Activity, 7(4), 273–290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S.C. (2006). On the human “interaction engine”. In N.J. Enfield & S.C. Levinson (Eds.), Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and interaction (pp. 39–69). Oxford: Berg.Google Scholar
Levitan, R., & Hirschberg, J. (2011). Measuring acoustic-prosodic entrainment with respect to multiple levels and dimensions. In Proceedings of INTERSPEECH 2011 (pp.3081–3084).
Lewis, K., Lange, D., & Gillis, L. (2005). Transactive memory systems, learning, and learning transfer. Organization Science, 16(6), 581–598. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Loftus, E.F. (1979). The malleability of human memory. American Scientist, 671, 312–320.Google Scholar
. (2005). Planting misinformation in the human mind: A 30-year investigation of the malleability of memory. Learning and Memory, 12(4), 361–366. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Louwerse, M.M., Dale, R., Bard, E.G., & Jeuniaux, P. (2012). Behavior matching in multimodal communication is synchronized. Cognitive Science, 361, 1404–1426. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luhmann, C.C., & Rajaram, S. (2013). Mnemonic diffusion: An agent-based modeling investigation of collective memory. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, & I. Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 936–941). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Malafouris, L. (2010). Metaplasticity and the human becoming: Principles of neuroarchaeology. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 881, 49–72.Google Scholar
. (2013). How things shape the mind: A theory of material engagement. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mason, M.F., Hood, B.M., & Macrae, C.N. (2004). Look into my eyes: Gaze direction and person memory. Memory, 121, 637–643. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meade, M.L., & Roediger, H.L., III. (2002). Explorations in the social contagion of memory. Memory & Cognition, 301, 995–1009. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meade, M.L., Nokes, T.J., & Morrow, D.G. (2009). Expertise promotes facilitation on a collaborative memory task. Memory, 171, 39–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2008). An experimental simulation of the “copy-successful-individuals” cultural learning strategy: Adaptive landscapes, producer-scrounger dynamics and informational access costs. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(5), 350–363. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mesoudi, A., & Whiten, A. (2004). The hierarchical transformation of event knowledge in human cultural transmission. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 4(1), 1–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2008). The multiple roles of cultural transmission experiments in understanding human cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 3631, 3489–3501. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Michaelian, K., & Sutton, J. (2013). Distributed cognition and memory research: History and current directions. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 4(1), 1–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Middleton, D., & Brown, S.D. (2005). The social psychology of experience: Studies in remembering and forgetting. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Muller, F., & Hirst, W. (2014). Remembering stories together: Social contagion and the moderating influence of disagreements in conversations. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 31, 7–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mühlhoff, R. (2014). Affective resonance and social interaction. Phenomenology and the Cogntive Sciences. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nomikou, I., Rohlfing, K.J., & Szufnarowska, J. (2013). Educating attention: Recruiting, maintaining and framing eye-contact in early natural mother-infant interactions. Interaction Studies, 14(2), 240–267. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Numbers, K.T., Meade, M.L., & Perga, V.A. (2014). The influences of partner accuracy and partner memory ability on social false memories. Memory & Cognition. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ost, J., & Costall, A. (2002). Misremembering Bartlett: A study in serial reproduction. British Journal of Psychology, 931, 243–255. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pacherie, E. (2013). How does it feel to act together? Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 13(1), 25–46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paxton, A., & Dale, R. (2013). Multimodal networks for interpersonal interaction and conversational contexts. In M. Knauff, M. Paun, N. Sebanz, & I Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp.1121–1126). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Pereira-Pasarin, L., & Rajaram, S. (2011). Study Repetition and divided attention: Effects of encoding manipulations on collaborative inhibition in group recall. Memory & Cognition, 391, 968–997. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M.J., & Garrod, S. (2004). Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 271, 169–190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2006). Alignment as the basis for successful communication. Research on Language and Computation, 4(2–3), 203–228. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2013). Forward models and their implications for production, comprehension, and dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 361, 377–392. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rączaszek-Leonardi, J., & Cowley, S.J. (2012). The evolution of language as controlled collectivity. Interaction Studies, 13(1), 1–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rajaram, S. (2011). Collaboration both hurts and helps memory: A cognitive perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 201, 76–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rajaram, S., & Pereira-Pasarin, L.P. (2010). Collaborative memory: Cognitive research and theory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 51, 649–663. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ren, Y., & Argote, A. (2011). Transactive memory systems 1985–2010: An integrative framework of key dimensions, antecedents and consequences. The Academy of Management Annals, 5(1), 189–229. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Richardson, D.C., Dale, R., & Kirkham, N.Z. (2007). The art of conversation is coordination common ground and the coupling of eye movements during dialogue. Psychological Science, 18(5), 407–413. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roediger, H.L. III, Meade, M.L., & Bergman, E. (2001). Social contagion of memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 81, 365–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roediger, H.L. III., & McDermott, K.B. (2011). Remember when? Science, 3331, 47–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roediger H.L. III, Meade, M.L., Gallo, D.A., & Olson, K.R. (2014). Bartlett revisited: Direct comparison of repeated reproduction and serial reproduction techniques. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 3(4), 266–271. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sato, W., & Yoshikawa, S. (2007). Spontaneous facial mimicry in response to dynamic facial expressions. Cognition, 1041, 1–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sauter, A., Uttal, D., Alman., A.S.,Goldin-Meadow, S., & Levine, S.C. (2012). Learning what children know about space from looking at their hands: The added value of gesture in spatial communication. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(4), 587–606. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, R.C., Morr, S., Fitzpatrick, P.A., & Richardson, M.J. (2012). Measuring the dynamics of interactional synchrony. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 361, 263–279. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shockley, K., Baker, A.A., Richardson, M.J., & Fowler, C.A. (2007). Articulatory constraints on interpersonal postural coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 331, 201–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sparrow, B., Liu, J., & Wegner, D.M. (2011). Google effects on memory: Cognitive consequences of having information at our fingertips. Science, 3331, 776–778. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Steffensen, S.V., & Pedersen, S.B. (2014). Temporal dynamics in human interaction. Cybernetics & Human Knowing, 21(1–2), 80–97.Google Scholar
Steffensen, S.V., Uryu, M., & Kramsch, C. (2014). The ecology of intercultural interactions: Timescales, temporal rangers and identity dynamics. Language Sciences, 41A1, 41–59.Google Scholar
Stel, M., van Baaren, R.B., & Vonk, R. (2008). Effects of mimicking: Acting prosocially by being emotionally moved. European Journal of Social Psychology, 381, 965–976. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2012). The evolved apprentice: How evolution made humans unique. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stevanoni, E., & Salmon, K. (2005). Giving memory a hand: Instructing children to gesture enhances their event recall. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 291, 217–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stone, C.B., & Hirst, W. (2014). (Induced) Forgetting to form a collective memory. Memory Studies, 7(3), 314–327. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stone, C., Coman, A., Brown, A.D., Koppel, J., & Hirst, W. (2012). Toward a science of silence: The consequences of leaving a memory unsaid. Perspectives in Psychological Science, 7(1), 39–53. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M.C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 301, 299–313. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sutton, J. (2008). Between individual and collective memory: Interaction, coordination, distribution. Social Research, 75(1), 23–48.Google Scholar
. (2013). Skill and collaboration in the evolution of human cognition. Biological Theory, 8(1), 28–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sutton, J., Harris, C.B., Keil, P.G., & Barnier, A.J. (2010). The psychology of memory, extended cognition, and socially distributed remembering. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 91, 521–560. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tan, R., & Fay, N. (2011). Cultural transmission in the laboratory: Agent interaction improves the intergenerational transfer of information. Evolution & Human Behavior, 32(6), 399–406. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Theiner, G. (2013). Transactive memory systems: A mechanistic analysis of emergent group memory. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 4(1), 65–89. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tollefsen, D.P., & Dale, R. (2012). Naturalizing joint action: A process-based approach. Philosophical Psychology, 25(3), 385–407. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tollefsen, D.P., Dale, R., & Paxton, A. (2013). Alignment, transactive memory and collective cognitive systems. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 4(1), 49–65. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (2005). First things first: Infants make good use of the sympathetic rhythm of imitation, without reason or language. Journal of Child Psychotherapy, 31(1), 91–113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wachsmuth, I., de Ruiter, J., Jaecks, P., & Kopp, S. (2013). Introduction: Why a new theory of communication? In I. Wachsmuth, J. de Ruiter, P. Jaecks, & S. Kopp (Eds.), Alignment in communication: Towards a new theory of communication (pp. 1–10). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wagoner, B., & Gillespie, A. (2013). Sociocultural mediators of remembering: An extension of Bartlett’s method of repeated reproduction. British Journal of Social Psychology, 53(4), 622–639. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wang, Q. (2013). The autobiographical self in time and culture. New York: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wegner, D.M. (1986). Transactive memory: A contemporary analysis of the group mind. In B. Mullen & G.R. Goethals (Eds.), Theories of group behavior (pp.185–208). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Wegner, D.M., Erber, R., & Raymond, P. (1991). Transactive memory in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 611, 923–929. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weldon, M.S., & Bellinger, K.D. (1997). Collective memory: Collaborative and individual processes in remembering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 231, 1160–1175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wertsch, J.V. (2002). Voices of collective remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge. University Press.. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wertsch, J.V. (2009). Collective memory. In P. Boyer & J.V. Wertsch (Eds.), Memory in mind and culture (pp. 117–137). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wheeler, M.A., & Roediger, H.L. III, (1992). Disparate effects of repeated testing: Reconciling Ballard’s (1913) and Bartlett’s (1932) results. Psychological Science, 31, 240–224. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wheeler, R., Allan, K., Tsivilis, D., Martin, D., & Gabbert, F. (2013). Explicit mentalizing mechanisms and their adaptive role in memory conformity. PLoS ONE, 8(4), e62106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Williamson, K., & Sutton, J. (2014). Embodied collaboration in small groups. In C.T. Wolfe (Ed.), Brain theory: Essays in critical neurophilosophy (pp.107–133). London: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, R.A. (2005). Collective memory, group minds, and the extended mind thesis. Cognitive Processing, 61, 227–236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Xu, J., & Griffiths, T.L. (2010). A rational analysis of the effects of memory biases on serial reproduction. Cognitive Psychology, 60(2), 107–126. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zerubavel, E. (2003). Time maps: Collective memory and the social shape of the past. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (18)

Cited by 18 other publications

daSilva, Elizabeth B. & Adrienne Wood
2024. How and Why People Synchronize: An Integrated Perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Review DOI logo
Bietti, Lucas M. & Eric Mayor
2023. A longitudinal study of conversational remembering in WhatsApp group messages before, during, and after COVID-19 lockdown. Memory, Mind & Media 2 DOI logo
Fawns, Tim
2023. Cued recall: Using photo-elicitation to examine the distributed processes of remembering with photographs. Memory Studies 16:2  pp. 264 ff. DOI logo
Fernandez Velasco, Pablo
2023. Entropy, prediction and the cultural ecosystem of human cognition. Synthese 201:3 DOI logo
Prezioso, Emanuele & Nicolás Alessandroni
2023. Enacting memories through and with things: Remembering as material engagement. Memory Studies 16:4  pp. 962 ff. DOI logo
Wahn, Basil, Peter König & Alan Kingstone
2023. Predicting group benefits in joint multiple object tracking. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 85:6  pp. 1962 ff. DOI logo
Gillett, Alexander James
2022. Development, Resilience Engineering, Degeneracy, and Cognitive Practices. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 13:3  pp. 645 ff. DOI logo
Wahn, Basil, Veera Ruuskanen, Alan Kingstone & Sebastiaan Mathôt
2021. Coordination effort in joint action is reflected in pupil size. Acta Psychologica 215  pp. 103291 ff. DOI logo
Wood, Adrienne, Jennie Lipson, Olivia Zhao & Paula Niedenthal
2021. Forms and Functions of Affective Synchrony. In Handbook of Embodied Psychology,  pp. 381 ff. DOI logo
Heersmink, Richard
2020. Narrative niche construction: memory ecologies and distributed narrative identities. Biology & Philosophy 35:5 DOI logo
Bietti, Lucas M. & Charles B. Stone
2019. Editors’ Introduction: Remembering With Others: Conversational Dynamics and Mnemonic Outcomes. Topics in Cognitive Science 11:4  pp. 592 ff. DOI logo
Norrick, Neal R.
2019. Collaborative Remembering in Conversational Narration. Topics in Cognitive Science 11:4  pp. 733 ff. DOI logo
Bietti, Lucas M & Michael J Baker
2018. Collaborating to remember collaborative design: An exploratory study. Memory Studies 11:2  pp. 225 ff. DOI logo
Bietti, Lucas M. & Michael J. Baker
2018. Collaborative remembering at work. Interaction Studies. Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systems 19:3  pp. 459 ff. DOI logo
Filho, Edson & Jean Rettig
2018. Team coordination in high-risk circus acrobatics. Interaction Studies. Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systems 19:3  pp. 499 ff. DOI logo
Hiss, Florian
2018. Talk, time, and creativity: Developing ideas and identities during a start-up weekend. Language & Communication 60  pp. 64 ff. DOI logo
Wahn, Basil, Artur Czeszumski, Peter König & Xiaoang Wan
2018. Performance similarities predict collective benefits in dyadic and triadic joint visual search. PLOS ONE 13:1  pp. e0191179 ff. DOI logo
Wahn, Basil, Alan Kingstone & Peter König
2018. Group benefits in joint perceptual tasks—a review. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1426:1  pp. 166 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 4 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.