Article published In:
Interaction Studies
Vol. 17:1 (2016) ► pp.125
References (44)
References
Abbink, K., Brandts, J., Herrmann, B., and Orzen, H. (2012). Parochial altruism in inter-group conflicts. Economics Letters, 117(1):45–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alexander, R. D. (1987). The Biology of Moral Systems. Foundations of Human Behavior. Aldine de Gruyter, New York.Google Scholar
Alizon, S. and Taylor, P. (2008). Empty sites can promote altruistic behaviour. Evolution, 62(6):1335–1344. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: I. a minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 70(9):1–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Axelrod, R. and Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. Science, 2111:1390–1396. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1976). Hierarchical organisation: A candidate principle for ethology. In Bateson, P. P. G. and Hinde, R. A., editors, Growing Points in Ethology, pages 7–54. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Dessalles, J.-L. (2007). Why we talk: the evolutionary origins of language. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Enquist, M. and Leimar, O. (1993). The evolution of cooperation in mobile organisms. Animal Behaviour, 451:747–757. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fahle, M. and Poggio, T. (1981). Visual hyperacuity: Spatiotemporal interpolation in human vision. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 213(1193):451–477. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., and Király, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415:755.Google Scholar
Gergely, G. and Csibra, G. (2006). Sylvia’s recipe: the role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of cultural knowledge, pages 229–255. Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition and Interaction. Berg, New York.Google Scholar
Goldberg, D. and Deb, K. (1991). A comparative analysis of selection schemes used in genetic algorithms. Urbana, 511:61801–2996.Google Scholar
Gruber, T., Poisot, T., Zuberbühler, K., Hoppitt, W., and Hobaiter, C. (2015). The spread of a novel behavior in wild chimpanzees: New insights into the ape cultural mind. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 8(2):e1017164. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hardin, G. (1968). The tragedy of the commons. Science, 1621:1243–1248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J. and McElreath, R. (2003). The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 12(3):123–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hess, N. and Hagen, E. (2006). Psychological adaptations for assessing gossip veracity. Human Nature, 17(3):337–354. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, R. A., Jordan, M. I., Nowlan, S. J., and Hinton, G. E. (1991). Adaptive mixtures of local experts. Neural computation, 3(1):79–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kao, A. B. and Couzin, I. D. (2014). Decision accuracy in complex environments is often maximized by small group sizes. Proceedings ofthe Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1784).Google Scholar
Kokko, H. (1997). Evolutionarily stable strategies of age-dependent sexual advertisement. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 41(2):99–107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McElreath, R. and Boyd, R. (2007). Mathematical models of social evolution: A guide for the perplexed. University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (1988). Infant imitation after a 1-week delay: Long-term memory for novel acts and multiple stimuli. Developmental psychology, 24(4):470. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Milinski, M., Semmann, D., and Krambeck, H.-J. (2006). Reputation helps solve the tragedy of the commons. Nature, 415(6870). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moravec, H. P. (1988). Sensor fusion in certainty grids for mobile robots. AI magazine, 9(2):61.Google Scholar
Nagell, K., Olguin, R. S., and Tomasello, M. (1993). Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (pan troglodytes) and human children (homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 107(2):174–186. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M. and Masuda, N. (2011). Indirect reciprocity under incomplete observation. PLoS Computational Biology, 7(7):e1002113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nowak, M. A. and Sigmund, K. (1998). Evolution of indirect reciprocity by image scoring. Nature, 3931:573–577. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005). Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature, 4371:1291–1298. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ohtsuki, H. and Iwasa, Y. (2006). The leading eight: Social norms that can maintain cooperation by indirect reciprocity. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 239(4):435–444. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ohtsuki, H., Iwasa, Y., and Nowak, M. A. (2009). Indirect reciprocity provides only a narrow margin of efficiency for costly punishment. Nature, 457(7225):79–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rand, D. G., Nowak, M. A., Fowler, J. H., and Christakis, N. A. (2014). Static network structure can stabilize human cooperation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(48):17093–17098. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rauwolf, P., Mitchell, D., and Bryson, J. J. (2015). Value homophily benefits cooperation but motivates employing incorrect social information. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 367(0):246–261. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Richerson, P. J. and Boyd, R. (2005). Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago University Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
Roberts, G. (2008). Evolution of direct and indirect reciprocity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275(1631):173–179. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schelling, T. (1960). The strategy of conflict. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Scott-Phillips, T. C. (2008). Defining biological communication. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 21(2):387–395. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sommerfeld, R. D., Krambeck, H. J., Semmann, D., and Milinski, M. (2007). Gossip as an alternative for direct observation in games of indirect reciprocity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(44):17435–17440. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. and Wilson, D. (1995). Relevance: Communication and Cognition. Wiley.Google Scholar
Taylor, D. J. (2014). Evolution of the Social Contract. PhD thesis, Department of Computer Science, University of Bath.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999). The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46(1):35–57. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Čače, I. and Bryson, J. J. (2007). Agent based modelling of communication costs: Why information can be free. In Lyon, C., Nehaniv, C. L., and Cangelosi, A., editors, Emergence and Evolution of Linguistic Communication, pages 305–322. Springer, London. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S., and Hopper, L. M. (2009). Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1528):2417–2428. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and Natural Selection. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Yoo, J. H. (2009). The power of sharing negative information in a dyadic context. Communication Reports, 22(1):29–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (2)

Cited by two other publications

Okafuji, Yuki, Jun Baba, Junya Nakanishi, Itaru Kuramoto, Kohei Ogawa, Yuichiro Yoshikawa & Hiroshi Ishiguro
2020. Can a humanoid robot continue to draw attention in an office environment?. Advanced Robotics 34:14  pp. 931 ff. DOI logo
Rudnicki, Konrad, Carolyn Declerck, Charlotte De Backer & Mario Berth
2019. Physiological changes during first encounters and their role in determining the perceived interaction quality. Interaction Studies. Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systems 20:2  pp. 275 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 18 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.