It is well-established that toddlers can correctly select a novel referent from an ambiguous array in response to a novel label. There is also a growing consensus that robust word learning requires repeated label-object encounters. However, the effect of the context in which a novel object is encountered is less well-understood. We present two embodied neural network replications of recent empirical tasks, which demonstrated that the context in which a target object is encountered is fundamental to referent selection and word learning. Our model offers an explicit account of the bottom-up associative and embodied mechanisms which could support children’s early word learning and emphasises the importance of viewing behaviour as the interaction of learning at multiple timescales.
Alvy Ray, S. (1978). Color gamut transform pairs. SIGGRAPH Computer Graphics, 12(3), 12–19
Axelsson, E. L., Churchley, K., & Horst, J. S. (2012). The right thing at the right time: Why ostensive naming facilitates word learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 31
Axelsson, E. L., & Horst, J. S. (2013). Testing a word is not a test of word learning. Acta Psychologica, 144(2), 264–268
Baldwin, D.A. (1993). Infants’ ability to consult the speaker for clues to word reference. Journal of Child Language, 20(02), 395–418
Bion, R. A., Borovsky, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Fast mapping, slow learning: Disambiguation of novel word–object mappings in relation to vocabulary learning at 18, 24, and 30months. Cognition, 126(1), 39–53
Borovsky, A., Ellis, E. M., Evans, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (2015). Lexical leverage: category knowledge boosts real-time novel word recognition in 2-year-olds. Developmental Science
Borovsky, A., & Elman, J. (2006). Language input and semantic categories: A relation between cognition and early word learning. Journal of Child Language, 33(04), 759–790
Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2005). The development of gaze following and its relation to language. Developmental Science, 8(6), 535–543
Cangelosi, A., & Schlesinger, M. (2015). Developmental robotics: From babies to robots. MIT Press.
Carey, S., & Bartlett, E. (1978). Acquiring a single new word. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 151, 17–29.
Clark, E.V. (1990). On the pragmatics of contrast. Journal of Child Language, 17(2), 417–431
Clark, E.V. (1995). The Lexicon in Acquisition. Cambridge University Press.
Fantz, R.L. (1964). Visual experience in infants: Decreased attention familar patterns relative to novel ones. Science, 1461(668–670
Faubel, C., & Schoner, G. (2008). Learning to recognize objects on the fly: A neurally based dynamic field approach. Neural Networks, 21(4), 562–576
Fazly, A., Alishahi, A., & Stevenson, S. (2010). A probabilistic computational model of cross-situational word learning. Cognitive Science, 34(6), 1017–1063
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., … Reilly, J. S. (1993). The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User’s Guide and Technical Manual. San Diego: Singular Publishing Group.
Fitneva, S. A., & Christiansen, M. H. (2011). Looking in the wrong direction correlates with more accurate word learning. Cognitive Science, 35(2), 367–380
Gillette, J., Gleitman, H., Gleitman, L., & Lederer, A. (1999). Human simulations of vocabulary learning. Cognition, 73(2), 135–176
Gliozzi, V., Mayor, J., Hu, J. F., & Plunkett, K. (2009). Labels as features (not names) for infant categorization: A neurocomputational approach. Cognitive Science, 33(4), 709–738
Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Bailey, L. M., & Wenger, N. R. (1992). Young children and adults use lexical principles to learn new nouns. Developmental Psychology, 28(1), 99–108
Golinkoff, R. M., Ma, W., Song, L., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2013). Twenty-five years using the Intermodal Preferential Looking Paradigm to study language acquisition: What have we learned?Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 316–339
Gurney, K. (1997). An introduction to neural networks. CRC Press.
Halberda, J. (2006). Is this a dax which I see before me? Use of the logical argument disjunctive syllogism supports word-learning in children and adults. Cognitive Psychology, 53(4), 310–44
Hebb, D. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory.
Horst, J. S., McMurray, B. & Samuelson, L. K. (2006). Online processing is essential for learning: Understanding fast mapping and word learning in a dynamic connectionist architecture. Proceedings of the Twenty-Eighth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Lawrence Erlbaum & Associates.
Horst, J. S., & Samuelson, L. K. (2008). Fast mapping but poor retention by 24-month-old infants. Infancy, 13(2), 128–157
Horst, J. S., Samuelson, L. K., Kucker, S. C., & McMurray, B. (2011). What’s new? Children prefer novelty in referent selection. Cognition, 118(2), 234–244
Horst, J. S., Scott, E. J., & Pollard, J. P. (2010). The role of competition in word learning via referent selection. Developmental Science, 13(5), 706–713
Horst, J. S., & Simmering, V. R. (2015). Category learning in a dynamic world. Frontiers in Psychology,6
Horst, J. S., McMurray, B. & Samuelson, L. K. (2006). Online processing is essential for learning: Understanding fast mapping and word learning in a dynamic connectionist architecture. Proceedings of the Twenty-Eighth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Lawrence Erlbaum & Associates
Houston-Price, C., & Nakai, S. (2004). Distinguishing novelty and familiarity effects in infant preference procedures. Infant and Child Development, 13(4), 341–348
Houston-Price, C., Plunkett, K., & Harris, P. (2005). “Word-learning wizardry” at 1;6. Journal of Child Language, 32(1), 175–189
Kohonen, T. (1998). The Self-Organizing Map, a possible model of brain maps. Brain and Values, 207–236 5681.
Klee, T., Marr, C., Robertson, E., & Harrison, C. (1999). The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory: Toddler (British English adaptation). Newcastle, England: Newcastle University Press.
Kucker, S. C., McMurray, B., & Samuelson, L. K. (2015). Slowing down fast mapping: Redefining the dynamics of word learning. Child Development Perspectives, 9(2), 74–78
Kucker, S. C., & Samuelson, L. K. (2011). The first slow step: Differential effects of object and word-form familiarization on retention of fast-mapped words. Infancy, 17(3), 295–323
Maestre, C., Cully, A., Gonzales, C., & Doncieux, S. (2015). Bootstrapping interactions with objects from raw sensorimotor data: a Novelty Search based approach. In Proceedings of the 5th International Conference on Development and Learning and on Epigenetic Robotics. Providence, RI.
Mani, N., & Plunkett, K. (2010). In the infant’s mind’s ear: Evidence for implicit naming in 18-month-olds. Psychological Science, 21(7), 908–913
Markman, E.M. (1990). Constraints children place on word meanings. Cognitive Science, 14(1), 57–77
Markman, E.M. (1994). Constraints on word meaning in early language acquisition. Lingua, 92(1–4), 199–227
Markman, E. M., & Hutchinson, J. E. (1984). Children’s sensitivity to constraints on word meaning: Taxonomic versus thematic relations. Cognitive Psychology, 16(1), 1–27
Markman, E. M., & Wachtel, G. F. (1988). Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meaning of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 121–157
Marocco, D., Cangelosi, A., Fischer, K., & Belpaeme, T. (2010). Grounding action words in the sensorimotor interaction with the world: experiments with a simulated iCub humanoid robot. Frontiers in Neurorobotics, 41
Mather, E., & Plunkett, K. (2009). Learning words over time: The role of stimulus repetition in Mutual Exclusivity. Infancy, 14(1), 60–76
McClelland, J. L. (2009). The place of modeling in cognitive science. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(1), 11–38
McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88(5), 375
McMurray, B., Horst, J. S., & Samuelson, L. K. (2012). Word learning emerges from the interaction of online referent selection and slow associative learning. Psychological Review, 119(4), 83877
Medina, T. N., Snedeker, J., Trueswell, J. C., & Gleitman, L. R. (2011). How words can and cannot be learned by observation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(22), 9014–9019
Merriman, W. E., Bowman, L. L., & MacWhinney, B. (1989). The mutual exclusivity bias in children’s word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, i–129
Mervis, C. B., & Bertrand, J. (1994). Acquisition of the Novel Name Nameless Category (N3c) principle. Child Development, 65(6), 1646–1662
Metta, G., Natale, L., Nori, F., Sandini, G., Vernon, D., Fadiga, L., von Hofsten, C., Rosander, K., Lopes, M., Santos-victor, J., Bernardino, A., & Montesano, L. (2010). The iCub humanoid robot: An open-systems platform for research in cognitive development. Neural Networks Neural Networks, 23(8–9), 1125–1134
Montesano, L., Lopes, M., Bernardino, A., & Santos-Victor, J. (2008). Learning object affordances: from sensory–motor coordination to imitation. IEEE Transactions on Robotics, 24(1), 15–26
Moore, R., Mueller, B., Kaminski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Two-year-old children but not domestic dogs understand communicative intentions without language, gestures, or gaze. Developmental Science, 18(2), 232–242
Morse, A. F., Benitez, V. L., Belpaeme, T., Cangelosi, A., & Smith, L. B. (2015). Posture affects how robots and infants map words to objects. PLoS ONE, 10(3), e0116012
Morse, A. F. & Cangelosi, A. (in press). Why are there developmental stages in language learning? A developmental robotics model of language development. Cognitive Science.
Morse, A. F., de Greeff, J., Belpeame, T., & Cangelosi, A. (2010). Epigenetic Robotics Architecture (ERA). IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development, 2(4), 325–339.
Munakata, Y., & Pfaffly, J. (2004). Hebbian learning and development. Developmental Science, 7(2), 141–8.
Munro, N., Baker, E., McGregor, K., Docking, K., & Arculi, J. (2012). Why word learning is not fast. Frontiers in Psychology, 31
Oudeyer, P.-Y., & Smith, L. (2016). How evolution may work through curiosity-driven developmental process. Topics in Cognitive Science, 81(2),492–502.
Perry, L. K., & Samuelson, L. K. (2011). The shape of the vocabulary predicts the shape of the bias. Frontiers in Psychology, 21, 345
Quine, W. V. O. (1960). Word and Object. Cambridge: MA: MIT Press.
Samuelson, L.K. (2002). Statistical regularities in vocabulary guide language acquisition in connectionist models and 15–20-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 38(6), 1016–1037
Samuelson, L. K., Smith, L. B., Perry, L. K., & Spencer, J. P. (2011). Grounding word learning in space. Plo S One, 6(12), e28095
Shaw, P., Law, J., & Lee, M. (2014). A comparison of learning strategies for biologically constrained development of gaze control on an icub robot. Autonomous Robots, 37(1), 97–110
Smith, L.B. (2000). Learning how to learn words: An associative crane. In R. M. Golinkoff, Hirsh-Pasek, K. , Bloom, L., Smith, L. B. , Woodward, A. L., Akhtar, N., … G. Hollich (Eds.), Becoming a Word Learner: A Debate on Lexical Acquisition (pp. 51–80). New York: Oxford University Press.
Smith, L. B., Colunga, E., & Yoshida, H. (2010). Knowledge as process: Contextually cued attention and early word learning. Cognitive Science, 34(7), 1287–1314
Smith, L. B., Jones, S. S., Yoshida, H., & Colunga, E. (2003). Whose DAM account? Attentional learning explains Booth and Waxman. Cognition, 87(3), 209–213.
Smith, L. B., & Yu, C. (2008). Infants rapidly learn word-referent mappings via cross-situational statistics. Cognition, 106(3), 1558–1568
Smith, L. B., Yu, C., & Pereira, A. F. (2011). Not your mother’s view: The dynamics of toddler visual experience. Developmental Science, 14(1), 9–17
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
Tikhanoff, V., Cangelosi, A., & Metta, G. (2011). Integration of speech and action in humanoid robots: iCub simulation experiments. Autonomous Mental Development, IEEE Transactions on, 3(1), 17–29
Tomasello, M., & Akhtar, N. (1995). Two-year-olds use pragmatic cues to differentiate reference to objects and actions. Cognitive Development, 10(2), 201–224
Twomey, K. E., Chang, F., & Ambridge, B. (2014). Do as I say, not as I do: A lexical distributional account of English locative verb class acquisition. Cognitive Psychology, 731, 41–71
Twomey, K. E., Horst, J. S., & Morse, A. F. (2013). An embodied model of young children’s categorization and word learning. In L. J. Gogate & G. Hollich (Eds.), Theoretical and Computational Models of Word Learning: Trends in Psychology and Artificial Intelligence (pp. 172–196). Hershey, PA: Information Science Reference
Twomey, K. E., Ranson, S. L., & Horst, J. S. (2014). That’s more like it: Multiple exemplars facilitate word learning. Infant and Child Development, 23(2), 105–122
Westermann, G., & Mareschal, D. (2012). Mechanisms of developmental change in infant categorization. Cognitive Development, 27(4), 367–382
Westermann, G., & Mareschal, D. (2014). From perceptual to language-mediated categorization. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1634), 20120391.
Yuan, S., Fisher, C., & Snedeker, J. (2012). Counting the nouns: Simple structural cues to verb neaning. Child Development, 83(4), 1382–1399
Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2007). Rapid word learning under uncertainty via cross-situational statistics. Psychological Science, 18(5), 414–420
Yurovsky, D., Fricker, D. C., Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2014). The role of partial knowledge in statistical word learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(1), 1–22
Zosh, J. M., Brinster, M., & Halberda, J. (2013). Optimal contrast: Competition between two referents improves word learning. Applied Developmental Science, 17(1), 20–28
Cited by (11)
Cited by 11 other publications
Hilton, Matt, Katherine E. Twomey & Gert Westermann
2024. Caregivers as experimenters: Reducing unfamiliarity helps shy children learn words. Infancy 29:6 ► pp. 877 ff.
Rothwell, Charlotte, Gert Westermann & Calum Hartley
2024. How do Autistic and Neurotypical Children’s Interests Influence their Accuracy During Novel Word Learning?. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 54:9 ► pp. 3301 ff.
Mohamed, Youssef & Severin Lemaignan
2021. 2021 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems (IROS), ► pp. 3020 ff.
2017. Cross-Situational Learning with Bayesian Generative Models for Multimodal Category and Word Learning in Robots. Frontiers in Neurorobotics 11
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 18 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.