Article published In:
Interaction Studies
Vol. 17:2 (2016) ► pp.306320
References
Cirelli, L.K., Einarson, K.M., & Trainor, L.J.
(2014) Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behaviour in infants. Developmental Science, 1–9, DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clayton, M.
(2009) The social and personal functions of music in cross-cultural perspective. S. Hallam, I. Cross & Thaut (Eds), The Oxford handbook of music psychology (pp 35–44). Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Costa, M., Fine, P., & Ricci Bitti, P. E.
(2004) Interval distributions, mode, and tonal strength of melodies as predictors of perceived emotion. Music Perception, 22(1), 1–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cross, I.
(2001) Music, cognition, culture, and evolution. Annals of the New York Academy of sciences, 930(1), 28–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cross, I., Fitch, W. T., Aboitiz, F., Iriki, A., Jarvis, E.D., Lewis, J., Liebal, K., Merker, B., Stout, D., & Trehub, S. E.
(2013) Culture and evolution. In M. A. Arbib (Ed.), Language, Music, and the Brain: A mysterious relationship (pp 541–562). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cross, I. and Woodruff, G. E.
(2009) Music as a communicative medium. In R. Botha and C. Knight (Eds.), The prehistory of language (pp 113–144). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dalla Bella, S., Peretz, I., Rousseau, L. and Gosselin, N.
(2001) A developmental study of the affective value of tempo and mode in music. Cognition, 80(1), 1–9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eerola, T., Friberg, A., & Bresin, R.
(2013) Emotional expression in music: contribution, linearity, and additivity of primary musical cues. Frontiers in Psychology, 41, 487. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Falk, D.
(2004) Prelinguistic evolution in early hominins: Whence motherese?. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(1), 491–503. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Franco, F., Swaine, J.S., Israni, S., Zaborowska, K.A., Kaloko, F., Kesavarajan, I. and Majek, J.A.
(2014) Affect-matching music improves cognitive performance in adults and young children for both positive and negative emotions. Psychology of Music. 42(1), 869–887. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gabrielsson, A., and Lindström, E.
(2010) The role of structure in the musical expression of emotions. In P. N. Juslin & J. Sloboda (Eds), Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, applications (pp. 367–400). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hove, M.J., and Risen, J.L.
(2009) It’s all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Social Cognition, 27(1), 949–961. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huron, D., & Davis, M.
(2013) The harmonic minor scale provides an optimum way of reducing average melodic interval size, consistent with sad affect cues. Empirical Musicology Review, 71(3–4), 103–117. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Juslin, P. N., & Lindström, E.
(2010) Musical expression of emotions: Modelling listeners’ judgements of composed and performed features. Music Analysi, 29(1–3), 334–364. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Juslin, P., and Timmers, R.
(2010) Expression and communication of emotion in music performance. Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, applications (pp. 453–489). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Juslin, P. N., & Sloboda, J.
(Eds.) (2011) Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, applications. OUP Oxford..Google Scholar
Juslin, P., and Västfjäll, D.
(2008) Emotional responses to music: The need to consider underlying mechanisms. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 31(1), 559–621. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Karageorghis, C. I., Priest, D. L., Williams, L. S., Hirani, R. M., Lannon, K. M. and Bates, B. J.
(2010) Ergogenic and psychological effects of synchronous music during circuit-type exercise. Psychology of Sport ad Exercise, 11(1), 551–559. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kenntner-Mabiala, R., Gorges, S., Alpers, G.W., Lehmann, A. C., and Pauli, P.
(2007) Musically induced arousal affects pain perception in females but not in males: A psychophysiological examination. Biological Psychology, 75(1), 19–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kirschner, S., and Tomasello, M.
(2010) Joint music-making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 311, 354–364. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koelsch S., Maess B., Grossmann T. and Friederici, A. D.
(2003) Electric brain responses reveal gender differences in music processing. Neuro Report, 14(1), 709–713.Google Scholar
Koelsch, S., Grossmann, T., Gunter, T.C., Hahne, A., Schroger, E., and Friederici, A.D.
(2003) Children Processing Music: Electric Brain Responses Reveal Musical Competence and Gender Differences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(1), 683–693. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroeger, K.
(2000) Early American Anthems: Part 1, Anthems for public celebrations. Madison: A-R Editions Inc.Google Scholar
Lakens, D., and Stel, D.
(2011) If they move in sync, they must feel in sync: Movement in synchrony leads to attributions of rapport and entitativity. Social Cognition, 29(1), 1–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Cheng, C. M. and Chartrand, T.
(2003) The chameleon effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behaviour, 27(1), 145–162. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Large, E. W., & Jones, M. R.
(1999) The dynamics of attending: how people track time-varying events. Psychological Review, 106(1), 119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lee D. J, Chen Y., and Schlaug G.
(2003) Corpus callosum: musician and gender effects. NeuroReport, 14(1), 205–209. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lewis, J.
(2009) 13 As well as words: Congo Pygmy hunting, mimicry, and play. The cradle of language, 121, 236.Google Scholar
(2013) A cross-cultural perspective on the significance of music and dance to culture and society. Insight from BaYaka Pygmies. In M.A. Arbib (ed.), Language, Music, and the Brain: A mysterious relationship (pp 45–65). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Macrae, C. N., Duffy, O. K., Miles, L. K. and Lawrence, J.
(2008) A case of hand waving: Action synchrony and person perception. Cognition, 109(1), 152–156. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNeil, H.
(1995) Keeping together in time: Dance and drill in human history. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Merker, B. H., Madison, G. S., & Eckerdal, P.
(2009) On the role and origin of isochrony in human rhythmic entrainment. Cortex, 45(1), 4–17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peretz, I.
(2006) The nature of music from a biological perspective. Cognition, 100(1), 1–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peretz, I., Gagnon, L., & Bouchard, B.
(1998) Music and emotion: perceptual determinants, immediacy, and isolation after brain damage. Cognition, 68(1), 111–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rabinowitch, T., Cross, I., and Burnard, P.
(2012) Long-term musical group interaction has a positive influence on empathy in children. Psychology of Music, 41(1), 484–498.Google Scholar
Schachner, A.
(2010) Auditory-motor entrainment in vocal mimicking species: additional ontogenetic and phylogenetic factors. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 3(1), 290–293. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sloboda, J. A., and O’Neil, S. A.
(2001) Emotions in everyday listening to music. In P. Juslin and J. Sloboda (eds), Music and emotion: Theory and research (pp. 415–429). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Takahashi, D. Y., Narayanan, D. Z., & Ghazanfar, A. A.
(2013) Coupled oscillator dynamics of vocal turn-taking in monkeys. Current Biology, 23(1), 2162–2168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trehub, S. E., Becker, J., & Morley, I.
(2015) Cross-cultural perspectives on music and musicality. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 370(1), 20140096.Google Scholar
Valdesolo, P., and DeSteno, D.A.
(2011) Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. Emotion, 11(1), 262–266. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Webster, G. D., and Weir, C. G.
(2005) Emotional Responses to Music: Interactive Effects of Mode, Texture, and Tempo. Motivation and Emotion, 29(1), 19–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, M., & Quinlan, P.
(1987) MRC Psycholinguistic Database: Machine Usable Dictionary, Version 2.00.Google Scholar
Wiltermuth, S. S., & Heath, C.
(2009) Synchrony and cooperation. Psychological Science, 20(1), 1–5. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Woolhouse, M., and Tidhar, D.
(2010) Group dancing leads to increased person-perception. In S.M. Demorest, S.J. Morrison, & P.S. Campbell (Eds.), Proceedings of the 11th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (ICMPC11). Seattle, Washington, USA.Google Scholar
Zentner, M., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R.
(2008) Emotions evoked by the sound of music: characterization, classification, and measurement. Emotion, 8(1), 494. DOI logoGoogle Scholar