Article published In:
How the Brain Got Language: Towards a New Road Map
Edited by Michael A. Arbib
[Interaction Studies 19:1/2] 2018
► pp. 136150
References
Agamaite, J. A., Chang, C. -J., Osmanski, M. S., & Wang, X.
(2015) A quantitative acoustic analysis of the vocal repertoire of the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 138(5), 2906–2928. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A.
(2012) How the brain got language: The mirror system hypothesis (Vol. 161). OUP USA. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bardi, M., Petto, A. J., & Lee‐Parritz, D. E.
(2001) Parental failure in captive cotton‐top tamarins (Saguinus Oedipus). American Journal of Primatology, 54(3), 159–169. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bezerra, B. M., & Souto, A.
(2008) Structure and usage of the vocal repertoire of Callithrix jacchus . International Journal of Primatolgoy (29), 671–701. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borjon, J. I., & Ghazanfar, A. A.
(2014) Convergent evolution of vocal cooperation without convergent evolution of brain size. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 84(2), 93–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, G. R., Almond, R. E. A., & van Bergen, Y.
(2004) Begging, stealing and offering: food transfer in non-human primates. Advances in the Study of Behaviour, 341, 265–295. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burkart, J., Kupferberg, A., Glasauer, S., & van Schaik, C.
(2012) Even simple forms of social learning rely on intention attribution in marmoset monkeys (Callithrix jacchus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(2), 129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burkart, J. M., Allon, O., Amici, F., Fichtel, C., Finkenwirth, C., Heschl, A., … van Schaik, C. P.
(2014) The evolutionary origin of human hyper-cooperation. Nature Communications, 5, 4747.Google Scholar
Burkart, J. M., Hrdy, S. B., & van Schaik, C. P.
(2009) Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 181, 175–186. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burkart, J. M., Schubiger, M. N., & van Schaik, C. P.
(2017) The evolution of general intelligence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1–65.Google Scholar
Burkart, J. M., & van Schaik, C. P.
(2010) Cognitive consequences of cooperative breeding in primates. Animal Cognition, 131, 1–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) The cooperative breeding perspective helps pinning down when uniquely human evolutionary processes are necessary Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, e34.Google Scholar
Caine, N. G., Addington, R. L., & Windfelder, T. L.
(1995) Factors affecting the rates of food calls given by red-bellied tamarins. Animal Behaviour, 50(1), 53–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Campbell, M., & Snowdon, C. T.
(2007) Vocal response of captive-reared Saguinus oedipus during mobbing. International Journal of Primatology, 28(2), 257–270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caro, T. M., & Hauser, M. D.
(1992) Is there teaching in nonhuman animals? Quarterly Review of Biology, 671, 151–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cäsar, C., & Zuberbühler, K.
(2012) Referential alarm calling behaviour in New World primates. Current Zoology, 58(5), 680–697. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chisholm, J. S.
(2003) Uncertainty, contingency, and attachment: A life history theory of Theory of Mind. In K. Sterelny & J. Fitness (Eds.), From mating to mentality. Evaluating Evolutionary Psychology. (pp. 125–154). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Chow, C. P., Mitchell, J. F., & Miller, C. T.
(2015) Vocal turn-taking in a non-human primate is learned during ontogeny. Proc. R. Soc. B, 282(1807), 20150069. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clay, Z., Smith, C. L., & Blumstein, D. T.
(2012) Food-associated vocalizations in mammals and birds: what do these calls really mean? Animal Behaviour, 83(2), 323–330. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cleveland, J., & Snowdon, C. T.
(1982) The complex vocal repertoire of the adult cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus oedipus). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 581, 231–270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de la Torre, S., & Snowdon, C. T.
(2009) Dialects in pygmy marmosets? Population variation in call structure. American Journal of Primatology, 71(4), 333–342. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dell’Mour, V., Range, F., & Huber, L.
(2009) Social learning and mother’s behavior in manipulative tasks in infant marmosets. American Journal of Primatology, 71(6), 503–509. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Digby, L. J., Ferrari, S. F., & Saltzman, W.
(2007) Callitrichines: The role of competition in cooperatively breeding species. In C. J. Campbell, A. Fuentes, K. C. MacKinnon, M. A. Panger, & S. K. Bearder (Eds.), Primates in perspective (pp. 85–105). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Eliades, S. J., & Miller, C. T.
(2016) Marmoset vocal communication: Behavior and neurobiology. Developmental neurobiology.Google Scholar
Elowson, A. M., Snowdon, C. T., & Lazaro-Perea, C.
(1998a) ‘Babbling’ and social context in infant monkeys: parallels to human infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 35–43.Google Scholar
(1998b) Infant ‘babbling’ in a nonhuman primate: complex seqeunces of vocal behavior. Behaviour, 1351, 643–664. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. T.
(2005) The evolution of language: a comparative review. Biology and Philosopy, 201, 193–230. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garber, P. A.
(1997) One for all and breeding for one: cooperation and competition as a tamarin reproductive strategy. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 5(6), 187–199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grice, H. P.
(1975) Logic and conversation. In P. Cole (Ed.), Syntax and Semantics, Vol 31, (pp. 41–58). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gros-Louis, J.
(2004) The function of food-associated calls in white-faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus, from the perspective of the signaller. Animal Behaviour, 67(3), 431–440. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guerreiro Martins, E. M., & Burkart, J. M.
(2013) Common marmosets preferentially share difficult to obtain food items. Folia Primatologica, 84(3–5), 281–282.Google Scholar
in prep.). Teaching in marmosets:contingent on age or skill levels of immatures?
Guerreiro Martins, E. M., Moura, A. C., Finkenwirth, C., & Burkart, J. M.
subm). Food sharing in three species of callitrichid monkeys (Callithrix jacchus, Leontopithecus chrysomelas & Sanguinus midas): Individual differences and interspecific variation. Animal Behaviour.
Gultekin, Y. B., & Hage, S. R.
(2017) Limiting parental feedback disrupts vocal development in marmoset monkeys. Nature Communications, 81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawkes, K.
(2014) Primate sociality to human cooperation. Human Nature, 25(1), 28–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Horn, L., Scheer, C., Bugnyar, T., & Massen, J. J.
(2016) Proactive prosociality in a cooperatively breeding corvid, the azure-winged magpie (Cyanopica cyana). Biology letters, 12(10), 20160649. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hrdy, S.
(2005a) Comes the child before the man: how cooperative breeding and prolonged postweaning dependence shaped human potentials. In B. Hewlett & M. Lamb (Eds.), Hunter gatherer childhood. (pp. 65–91). Psicataway: Transactions.Google Scholar
(2005b) Evolutionary context of human development: The cooperative breeding model. In C. S. Carter, L. Ahnert, K. E. Grossmann, S. B. Hrdy, M. E. Lamb, S. W. Porges, & N. Sachser (Eds.), Attachment and Bonding: A New Synthesis; From the 92nd Dahlem Workshop Report (pp. 9–32): MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2009) Mothers & Others: The Evolutionary Origins of Mutual Understanding. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B.
(2016) Variable postpartum responsiveness among humans and other primates with “cooperative breeding”: A comparative and evolutionary perspective. Hormones and Behavior, 771, 272–283. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Humle, T., & Snowdon, C. T.
(2008) Socially biased learning in the acquisition of a complex foraging task in juvenile cottontop tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Animal Behaviour, 27(1), 267–277. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jaeggi, A., Burkart, J. M., & van Schaik, C. P.
(2010) On the psychology of cooperation in humans and other primates: The natural history of food sharing and experimental evidence of prosociality. PhilTransB 12(365), 2723–2735.Google Scholar
Kirchhof, J., & Hammerschmidt, K.
(2006) Functionally referential alarm calls in tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis and Saguinus mystax)–evidence from playback experiments. Ethology, 112(4), 346–354. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kitzmann, C. D., & Caine, N. G.
(2009) Marmoset (Callithrix geoffroyi) Food‐Associated Calls are Functionally Referential. Ethology, 115(5), 439–448. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kline, M. A.
(2014) How to learn about teaching: An evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behavior in humans and other animals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kupferberg, A., Glasauer, S., & Burkart, J. M.
(2013) Do robots have goals? How agent cues influence action understanding in non-human primates. Behavioural Brain Research. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leighton, G. M.
(2017) Cooperative breeding influences the number and type of vocalizations in avian lineages. Proc. R. Soc. B, 2841, 20171508. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacLean, E. L., Hare, B., Nunn, C. L., Addessi, E., Amici, F., Anderson, R. C., … Barnard, A. M.
(2014) The evolution of self-control. PNAS, 111(20), E2140–E2148. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacLean, E. L., Matthews, L. J., Hare, B., Nunn, C. L., Anderson, R. C., Aureli, F., … Emery, N. J.
(2012) How does cognition evolve? Phylogenetic comparative psychology. Animal Cognition, 15(2), 223–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Masataka, N.
(1982) A field study on the vocalizations of Goeldi’s monkeys (Callimico goeldii). Primates, 231, 206–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McComb, K., & Semple, S.
(2005) Coevolution of vocal communication and sociality in primates. Biology Letters, 1(4), 381–385. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Melis, A. P., & Warneken, F.
(2016) The psychology of cooperation: Insights from chimpanzees and children. Evolutionary Anthropology, 25(6), 297–305. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Milward, S. J., Kita, S., & Apperly, I. A.
(2016) Individual differences in children’s co-representation of self and other in joint action. Child Development.Google Scholar
Miss, F. M., & Burkart, J. M.
(2018) Corepresentation During Joint Action in Marmoset Monkeys (Callithrix jacchus). Psychological Science, DOI logo.Google Scholar
Moura, A. C., Nunes, H. G., & Langguth, A.
(2010) Food Sharing in Lion Tamarins (Leontopithecus chrysomelas): Does Foraging Difficulty Affect Investment in Young by Breeders and Helpers? International Journal of Primatology, 31(5), 848–862. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noble, J.
(2000) Cooperation, competition and the evolution of prelinguistic communication. The Emergence of Language, 40–61. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pistorio, A. L., Vintch, B., & Wang, X.
(2006) Acoustic analysis of vocal development in a New World primate, the common marmoet (Callithrix jacchus). J Aocust Soc Am, 120(3), 1655–1670. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pollick, A. S., Gouzoules, H., & de Waal, F. B. M.
(2005) Audience effects on food calls in captive brown capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella . Animal Behaviour, 701, 1273–1281. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rapaport, L. G.
(2011) Progressive parenting behavior in wild golden lion tamarins. Behavioral Ecology. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roupe, S. L., Pistorio, A., & Wang, X.
(2003) Vocal plasticity induced by auditory deprivation in the common marmoset. Paper presented at the Society for Neuroscience, New Orleans, Nov. 11.
Roy, S., Miller, C. T., Gottsch, D., & Wang, X.
(2011) Vocal control by the common marmoset in the presence of interfering noise. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 214(21), 3619–3629. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruch, H., Zürcher, Y., & Burkart, J. M.
(2018) The function of vocal accommodation in humans and other primates. Biological Reviews, 93(2), 996–1013. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rukstalis, M., Fite, J. E., & French, J. A.
(2003) Social Change Affects Vocal Structure in a Callitrichid Primate (Callithrix kuhlii). Ethology, 109(4), 327–340. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rukstalis, M., & French, J. A.
(2005) Vocal buffering of the stress response: exposure to conspecific vocalizations moderates urinary cortisol excretion in isolated marmosets. Hormones and Behavior, 47(1), 1–7. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shelley, E. L. & Blumstein, D. T.
(2004) The evolution of vocal alarm communication in rodents. Behavioral Ecology, 16(1), 169–77. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W.
(2003) Representing others’ actions: just like one’s own? Cognition, 88(3), B11–B21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snowdon, C. T.
(2013) Language parallels in New World primates Animal Models of Speech and Language Disorders (pp. 241–261). New York: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2001) Social processes in communication and cognition in callitrichid monkeys: a review. Animal Cognition, 41, 247–257. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017a) Cultural Phenomena in Cooperatively Breeding Primates. In J. M. Causadias, E. H. Telzer, & N. A. Gonzales (Eds.), The Handbook of Culture and Biology: John Wiley & Sons. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017b) Learning from monkey “talk”. Science, 355(6330), 1120–1122. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017c) Vocal Communication in Family-Living and Pair-Bonded Primates Primate Hearing and Communication (pp. 141–174): Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snowdon, C. T., & Cleveland, J.
(1984) “Conversations” among pygmy marmosets. American Journal of Primatology 71, 15–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snowdon, C. T., & Cronin, K. A.
(2007) Cooperative breeders do cooperate. Behavioural Processes, 76(2), 138–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snowdon, C. T., & Elowson, A. M.
(2001) ‘Babbling’ in pygmy marmosets: development after infancy. Behaviour, 138(10), 1235–1248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snowdon, C. T., & Roskos, T. R.
(2017) Stick-weaving: Innovative behavior in tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 131(2), 174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Solomon, N. G., & French, J. A.
(1997) Cooperative Breeding in Mammals. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Suzuki, W., Banno, T., Miyakawa, N., Abe, H., Goda, N., & Ichinohe, N.
(2015) Mirror neurons in a new world monkey, common marmoset. Frontiers in neuroscience, 91. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Takahashi, D. Y., Fenley, A. R., & Ghazanfar, A. A.
(2016) Early development of turn-taking with parents shapes vocal acoustics in infant marmoset monkeys. Phil. Trans. B, 371(1693), 20150370. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Takahashi, D. Y., Fenley, A. R., Teramoto, Y., Narayanan, D. Z., Borjon, J. I., Holmes, P., & Ghazanfar, A. A.
(2015) The developmental dynamics of marmoset monkey vocal production. Science, 349(6249), 734–738. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Takahashi, D. Y., Liao, D. A., & Ghazanfar, A. A.
(2017) Vocal Learning via Social Reinforcement by Infant Marmoset Monkeys. Current Biology. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Takahashi, D. Y., Narayanan, D. Z., & Ghazanfar, A. A.
(2013) Coupled oscillator dynamics of vocal turn-taking in monkeys. Current Biology, 23(21), 2162–2168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2017) What did we learn from the ape language studies?. In: Hare, B. & Yamamoto, S. (eds.) Bonobos: Unique in Mind, Brain, and Behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 95–105Google Scholar
(2008) Origins of human communication. Cambridge MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Gonzalez-Cabrera, I.
(2017) The role of ontogeny in the evolution of human cooperation. Human Nature, 1–15.Google Scholar
Vitale, A., Zanzoni, M., Queyras, A., & Chiarotti, F.
(2003) Degree of social contact affects the emission of food calls in the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) . American Journal of Primatology, 591, 21–28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Voelkl, B., & Huber, L.
(2007) Imitation as faithful copying of a novel techinque in marmoset monkeys. PLoS ONE, 2(7), e611. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weiss, D. J., Garibaldi, B. T., & Hauser, M. D.
(2001) The production and perception of long calls by cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): acoustic analyses and playback experiments. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115(3), 258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wen, T., & Hsieh, S.
(2015) Neuroimaging of the joint Simon effect with believed biological and non-biological co-actors. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 91. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K.
(2011) Cooperative breeding and the evolution of vocal flexibility. In M. Tallerman & K. Gibson (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Language Evolution.Google Scholar
Zürcher, Y., & Burkart, J. M.
(2017) Evidence for dialects in three captive populations of common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). International Journal of Primatolgoy, 38(4), 780–793. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
in prep). Translocation experiments provide evidence for vocal accommodation learing in marmosets.
Cited by

Cited by 25 other publications

Arbib, Michael A., Francisco Aboitiz, Judith M. Burkart, Michael Corballis, Gino Coudé, Erin Hecht, Katja Liebal, Masako Myowa-Yamakoshi, James Pustejovsky, Shelby Putt, Federico Rossano, Anne E. Russon, P. Thomas Schoenemann, Uwe Seifert, Katerina Semendeferi, Chris Sinha, Dietrich Stout, Virginia Volterra, Sławomir Wacewicz & Benjamin Wilson
2018. The comparative neuroprimatology 2018 (CNP-2018) road map for research on How the Brain Got Language . Interaction Studies. Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systems 19:1-2  pp. 370 ff. DOI logo
Arbib, Michael A., Francisco Aboitiz, Judith M. Burkart, Michael C. Corballis, Gino Coudé, Erin Hecht, Katja Liebal, Masako Myowa-Yamakoshi, James Pustejovsky, Shelby S. Putt, Federico Rossano, Anne E. Russon, P. Thomas Schoenemann, Uwe Seifert, Katerina Semendeferi, Chris Sinha, Dietrich Stout, Virginia Volterra, Sławomir Wacewicz & Benjamin Wilson
2020. The comparative neuroprimatology 2018 (CNP-2018) road map for research on How the Brain Got Language. In How the Brain Got Language – Towards a New Road Map [Benjamins Current Topics, 112],  pp. 370 ff. DOI logo
Boulinguez-Ambroise, Grégoire, Juliette Aychet & Emmanuelle Pouydebat
2022. Limb Preference in Animals: New Insights into the Evolution of Manual Laterality in Hominids. Symmetry 14:1  pp. 96 ff. DOI logo
Brügger, Rahel K. & Judith M. Burkart
2021. Parental reactions to a dying marmoset infant: conditional investment by the mother, but not the father. Behaviour 159:1  pp. 89 ff. DOI logo
Burkart, Judith M., Sandro Sehner, Rahel K. Brügger, Jessie E. C. Adriaense & Carel P. van Schaik
2023. Putting the cart before the horse? The origin of information donation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 46 DOI logo
Ghazanfar, Asif A., Lauren M. Kelly, Daniel Y. Takahashi, Sandra Winters, Rebecca Terrett & James P. Higham
2020. Domestication Phenotype Linked to Vocal Behavior in Marmoset Monkeys. Current Biology 30:24  pp. 5026 ff. DOI logo
Hrdy, Sarah Blaffer & Judith M. Burkart
2020. The emergence of emotionally modern humans: implications for language and learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 375:1803  pp. 20190499 ff. DOI logo
Hrdy, Sarah Blaffer & Judith M. Burkart
2022. How Reliance on Allomaternal Care Shapes Primate Development with Special Reference to the Genus Homo. In Evolutionary Perspectives on Infancy [Evolutionary Psychology, ],  pp. 161 ff. DOI logo
Lauer, Gerhard
2022. Language, Childhood, and Fire: How We Learned to Love Sharing Stories. Frontiers in Psychology 12 DOI logo
Leongómez, Juan David, Jan Havlíček & S. Craig Roberts
2022. Musicality in human vocal communication: an evolutionary perspective. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 377:1841 DOI logo
Manoochehri, Majid
2021. The Emergence of Language: a Review of Ljiljana Progovac (2019); A Critical Introduction to Language Evolution: Current Controversies and Future Prospects. Evolutionary Psychological Science 7:1  pp. 91 ff. DOI logo
Oller, D. Kimbrough & Ulrike Griebel
2021. Functionally Flexible Signaling and the Origin of Language. Frontiers in Psychology 11 DOI logo
Phaniraj, Nikhil, Kaja Wierucka, Yvonne Zürcher & Judith M. Burkart
2023. Who is calling? Optimizing source identification from marmoset vocalizations with hierarchical machine learning classifiers. Journal of The Royal Society Interface 20:207 DOI logo
Pleyer, Michael & Stefan Hartmann
2024. Cognitive Linguistics and Language Evolution, DOI logo
Prieur, Jacques, Stéphanie Barbu, Catherine Blois‐Heulin & Alban Lemasson
2020. The origins of gestures and language: history, current advances and proposed theories. Biological Reviews 95:3  pp. 531 ff. DOI logo
Proust, Joëlle
2023. Informational communication and metacognition. Evolutionary Linguistic Theory 5:1  pp. 11 ff. DOI logo
Richerson, Peter J., Sergey Gavrilets & Frans B. M. de Waal
2021.  Modern theories of human evolution foreshadowed by Darwin’s Descent of Man . Science 372:6544 DOI logo
Sehner, Sandro & Judith M. Burkart
2023. Cumulative Culture. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie 55:1  pp. 9 ff. DOI logo
Sehner, Sandro, Erik P Willems, Lucio Vinicus, Andrea B Migliano, Carel P van Schaik, Judith M Burkart & Karen E Nelson
2022. Problem-solving in groups of common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): more than the sum of its parts. PNAS Nexus 1:4 DOI logo
Slocombe, Katie E, Nicole J Lahiff, Claudia Wilke & Simon W Townsend
2022. Chimpanzee vocal communication: what we know from the wild. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 46  pp. 101171 ff. DOI logo
Tilston, Ottilie, Adrian Bangerter & Kristian Tylén
2022. Teaching, sharing experience, and innovation in cultural transmission. Journal of Language Evolution 7:1  pp. 81 ff. DOI logo
van Schaik, Carel P., Gauri R. Pradhan & Claudio Tennie
2019. Teaching and curiosity: sequential drivers of cumulative cultural evolution in the hominin lineage. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 73:1 DOI logo
Wacewicz, Sławomir & Przemysław Żywiczyński
2018. Language origins. Interaction Studies. Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systems 19:1-2  pp. 167 ff. DOI logo
Wacewicz, Sławomir & Przemysław Żywiczyński
2020. Language origins. In How the Brain Got Language – Towards a New Road Map [Benjamins Current Topics, 112],  pp. 167 ff. DOI logo
Zürcher, Yvonne, Erik P. Willems, Judith M. Burkart & Jon T. Sakata
2019. Are dialects socially learned in marmoset monkeys? Evidence from translocation experiments. PLOS ONE 14:10  pp. e0222486 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 31 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.