Article published In:
How the Brain Got Language: Towards a New Road Map
Edited by Michael A. Arbib
[Interaction Studies 19:1/2] 2018
► pp. 352369
References (93)
References
Alme, C. B., Miao, C., Jezek, K., Treves, A., Moser, E. I. & Moser, M. -B. (2014). Place cells in the hippocampus: Eleven maps for eleven rooms. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 1111, 18428–18435. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Babb, S. J. & Crystal, J. D. (2005). Discrimination of what, when, and where: Implications for episodic-like memory in rats. Learning & Motivation, 361, 177–189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barnard, A. (2013). Cognitive and social aspects of language origins. In Lefebvre, C., Comrie, B. &. Cohen, H. (Eds.), New perspectives on the origins of language (pp. 53–71). Amesterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beran, M. J. (2015). Animal memory: chimpanzees anticipate what comes next in short movies. Current Biology, 251, R827–R844 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beran, M. J., Perdue, B. M., Bramlett, J. L., Menzel, C. R. & Evans, T. A. (2012). Prospective memory in a language-trained chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Learning & Motivation, 431, 192–199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, D. (1990). Language and species. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014). More than nature needs: Language, mind, and evolution. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bischof-Kohler, D. (1985). Zur phyogenese menschlicher motivation [On the phylogeny of human motivation]. In L. H. Eckensberger & E. D. Lantermann (Eds.), Emotion und reflexivitut (pp. 3–47). Vienna: Urban & Schwarzenberg.Google Scholar
Bonnici, H. M., Chadwick, M. J., Lutti, A., Hassabis, D., Weiskopf, N. & Maguire, E. A. (2012). Detecting representations of recent and remote autobiographical memories in vmPFC and hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 16, 982–16, 991.Google Scholar
Buzsáki, G. (1989). Two-stage model of memory trace formation: A role for “noisy” brain states. Neuroscience, 311, 551–570. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive. Sciences, 121, 187–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2010). Some simple evo devo theses: How true might they be for language? In R. K. Larson, Déprez, V. & Yamakido, H. (Eds), The evolution of human language (pp. 45–62). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clayton, N. S., Bussey, T. J. & Dickinson, A. (2003). Can animals recall the past and plan for the future? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 41, 685–691.Google Scholar
Corballis, M. C. (2011). The recursive mind: The origins of human language, thought, and civilization. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
(2013). Mental time travel: A case for evolutionary continuity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 171, 5–6. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017). Language evolution: A changing perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 271, 229–236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Corkin, S. (2013). Permanent present tense: The man with no memory, and what he taught the world. London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Covington, N. V. & Duff, M. C. (2016). Expanding the language network: contributions from the hippocampus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 201, 869–870. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex, 2nd edition. London: John Murray. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dediu, D. & Levinson, S. C. (2013). On the antiquity of language: The reinterpretation of Neandertal linguistic capacities and its consequences. Frontiers in Psychology, 41, article 397. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Derdikman, D. & Moser, M. B. (2010). A dual role for hippocampal replay. Neuron, 651, 582–584. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deuker, L., Bellmund, J. L. S., Schröder, T. N. & Doeller, C. F. (2016). An event map of memory space in the hippocampus. eLife, 51, e16534. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Waal, F. B. M. (2012). The antiquity of empathy. Science, 3361, 874–876. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dor, D. (2015). The instruction of imagination: Language as a social communication technology. New York: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duff, M. C. & Brown-Schmidt, S. (2012). The hippocampus and the flexible use and processing of language. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 61, article 69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Emery, N. & Clayton, N. (2004). The mentality of crows: convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes. Science, 3061, 1903–1907. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fauconnier, G. (2003). Cognitive linguistics. In Nadel, L. (Ed.) Encyclopedia of cognitive science (pp. 539–543). London: Nature Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Ferkin, M. H., Combs, A., del Barco-Trillo, J., Pierce, A. A. & Franklin, S. (2008). Meadow voles, Microtus pennsylvanicus, have the capacity to recall the “what”, “where”, and “when” of a single past event. Animal Cognition, 111, 147–159. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foster, D. J. & Wilson, M. A. (2006). Reverse replay of behavioural sequences in hippocampal place cells during the awake state. Nature, 4401, 680–683. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gärdenförs, P. (2014). The evolution of sentential structure. Humana.Mente Journal of Philosophical Studies, 271, 79–97.Google Scholar
Gärdenförs, P. & Osvath, M. (2010). Prospection as a cognitive precursor to symbolic communication. In R. Larson, V. Déprez, & H. Yamakido (Eds.), Evolution of language: Biolinguistic approaches (pp. 103–114). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibson, B., Wilkinson, M. & Kelly, D. (2012). Let the pigeon drive the bus: pigeons can plan future routes in a room. Animal Cognition, 151, 379–391. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gordon, B. A., Shelton, J. T., Bugg, J. M., McDaniel, M. A. & Head, D. (2011). Structural correlates of prospective memory. Neuropsychologia, 491, 3795–3800. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1989). Studies in the ways of words. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Guazzelli, A., Corbacho, F. J., Bota, M., & Arbib, M. A. (1998). Affordances, motivation, and the world graph theory. Adaptive Behavior, 61, 435–471. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hafting, T., Fyhn, M., Molden, S., Moser, M. -B., & Moser, E. I. (2005). Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature, 4361, 801–806. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D. & Maguire, E. A. (2007). Using imagination to understand the neural basis of episodic memory. Journal of Neuroscience. 271, 14365–1437. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hasselmo, M. E. (2009). A model of episodic memory: Mental time travel along encoded trajectories using grid cells. Neurobiology of Learning & Memory, 921, 559–573. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hockett, C. F. (1960). The origins of speech. Scientific American, 203(3), 88–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hobaiter, C. & Byrne, R. W. (2011). Serial gesturing by wild chimpanzees: Its nature and function for communication. Animal Cognition, 141, 827–838. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hughes, J. K., Haywood, A., Mithen, S. J., Sellwood, B. W. & Valdes, P. J. (2007). Investigating early hominin dispersal patterns: developing a framework for climate data integration. Journal of Human Evolution, 531, 465–474. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Intraub, H. & Richardson, M. (1989). Wide-angle memories of close-up scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 151, 179–187.Google Scholar
Jacobs, J., Weidemann, C. T., Miller, J. F., Solway, A., Burke, J. F., Wei, X. -X.Kahana, M. J. (2013). Direct recordings of grid-like neuronal activity in human spatial navigation. Nature Neuroscience, 161, 1188–1190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Janmaat, K. R. L., Polansky, L., Ban, S. D. & Boesch, C. (2014). Wild chimpanzees plan their breakfast time, type, and location. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA,. 1111, 16343–16348. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johansson, S. (2013). The talking Neandertals: What do fossils, genetics, and archeology say? Biolinguistics, 71, 35–74.Google Scholar
Kabadayi, C. & Osvath, M. (2017). Ravens parallel great apes in flexible planning for tool-use and bartering. Science, 3571, 202-204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaminski, J., Call, J. & Fischer, J. (2004). Word learning in the domestic dog: evidence for ‘fast mapping’. Science 3041, 1682–1683. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kano, F., & Hirata, S. (2015). Great apes make anticipatory looks based on long-term memory of single events. Current Biology, 251, 2513–2517, DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Klein, S. B., Robertson, T. E., & Delton, A. W. (2010). Facing the future: Memory as an evolved system for planning future acts. Memory & Cognition, 381, 13–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Köhler, W. (1925). The mentality of apes. New York: Routledge & Kegan Paul. (Originally published in German in 1917).Google Scholar
Kundera, M. (2002). Ignorance. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Lieblich, I., & Arbib, M. A. (1982). Multiple representations of space underlying behavior. The Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 51, 627–659. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Loftus, E. & Ketcham, K. (1994). The myth of repressed memory. New York: St. Martin’s Press.Google Scholar
Maguire, E. A., Intraub, H. & Mullally, S. L. (2016). Scenes, spaces, and memory traces: What does the hippocampus do? The Neuroscientist, 221, 432–439. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martin, V. C., Schacter, D. L., Corballis, M. C. & Addis, D. R. (2011). A role for the hippocampus in encoding simulations of future events. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 1081, 13858–13863. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Menzel, C. R., Savage-Rumbaugh, S. & Menzel, E. W., Jr. (2002). Bonobo (Pan paniscus) spatial memory and communication in a 20-hectare forest. International Journal of Primatology, 231, 601–619. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miller, J. F., Neufang, M., Solway, A., Brandt, A., Trippel, M., Mader, I.Schulze-Bonhage, A. (2013). Neural activity in human hippocampal formation reveals the spatial context of retrieved memories. Science, 3421, 1111–1114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moser, M. B., Rowland, D. C. & Moser, E. I. (2015). Place cells, grid cells, and memory. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology. 71, a021808. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Keefe, J. & Nadel, N. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Osvath, M. & Karvonen, E. (2012). Spontaneous innovation for future deception in a male chimpanzee. PLoS ONE 71, e36782. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Patterson, F. G. P. & Gordon, W. (2001). Twenty-seven years of Project Koko and Michael.” In Galdikas, B., Briggs, N. E., Sheeran, L. K. & Goodall, J. (Eds.), All apes great and small, Vol. 1: African Apes (pp. 165–176). New York: Kluver.Google Scholar
Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J. & Povinelli, D. J. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral & Brain Sciences. 311, 108–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pfeiffer, B. E. & Foster, D. J. (2013). Hippocampal place-cell sequences depict future paths to remembered goals. Nature, 4971, 74–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Piai, V., Anderson, K. L., Lin, J. J., Dewar, C., Parvizi, J., Dronkers, N. F., & Knight, R. T. (2016). Direct brain recordings reveal hippocampal rhythm underpinnings of language processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 1131, 11366–11371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pilley, J. W. & Reid, A. K. (2011). Border collie comprehends object names as verbal referents. Behavioral Processes, 861, 184–195. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. & Jackendoff, R. (2005). The faculty of language: What’s special about it? Cognition, 951, 201–236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Premack, D. (2007). Human and animal cognition: Continuity and discontinuity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 1041, 13861–13867. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Premack, D. & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 41, 515–526. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Raffaele, P. (2006). Speaking bonobo. Smithsonian Magazine, November 2006. Online at: [URL].
Roberts, W. A. (2002). Are animals stuck in time? Psychological Bulletin, 1281, 473–489. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roediger, H. L. & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 24(4), 803–814Google Scholar
Rugg, M. D. & Vilberg, K. L. (2013). Brain networks underlying episodic memory retrieval. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 231, 255–260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Russon, A. & Andrews, K. (2001). Orangutan pantomime: Elaborating the message. Biology Letters, 71, 627–630. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schacter, D. L. (2012). Adaptive constructive processes and the future of memory. American Psychologist, 671, 603–613. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scott-Phillips, T. (2015). Speaking our minds: Why human communication is different, and how language evolved to make it special. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sebeok, T. A. & Rosenthal, R. (Eds.) (1981). The Clever Hans phenomenon: Communication with horses, whales, apes, and people. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M. & Cheney, D. L. (2017). Precursors to language: Social cognition and pragmatic inference in primates. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 241, 79–84. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Squire, L. R. (2004). Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 821. 171–177. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stout, D. & Chaminade, T. (2012). Stone tools, language and the brain in human evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 3671, 75–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stout, D. & Hecht, E. E. (2017). Evolutionary neuroscience of cumulative culture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 1141, 7861–7868 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Suddendorf, T. (2013). Mental time travel: continuities and discontinuities. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 171, 151–152. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Suddendorf, T. & Corballis, M. C. (1997). Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 1231, 133–167.Google Scholar
(2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 301, 299–351. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010). Behavioural evidence for mental time travel in nonhuman animals. Behavioural Brain Research, 2151, 292–298. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Timmermann, A. & Friedrich, T. (2016). Late Pleistocene climate drivers of early human migration. Nature, 5381, 92–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taylor, A. H., Hunt, G. R., Holzhaider, J. C. & Gray, R. D. (2007). Spontaneous metatool use by New Caledonian crows. Current Biology, 171, 1504–1507 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tooby, J., and DeVore, I. (1987). The reconstruction of hominid evolution through strategic modeling. In W. G. Kinzey (Ed.), The evolution of human behavior: Primate models (pp. 183–227). Albany, NY: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 531, 1–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Venkataraman, V. V., Kraft, T. S., Dominy, N. J., Endicott, K. M. (2017). Hunter-gatherer residential mobility and the marginal value of rainforest patches. Proceedings of the National academy of Sciences, 1141, 3097–3102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wearing, D. (2005). Forever today. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Wilson, A. G., Pizzo, M. J. & Crystal, J. D. (2013). Event-based prospective memory in the rat. Current Biology. 231, 1089–1093. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zinkivskay, A., Nazir, F. & Smulders, T. V. (2009). What–Where–When memory in magpies (Pica pica). Animal Cognition, 121, 119–125 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (1)

Cited by one other publication

Arbib, Michael
2024. Chapter 1. Pantomime within and beyond the evolution of language. In Perspectives on Pantomime [Advances in Interaction Studies, 12],  pp. 16 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 6 august 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.