This paper outlines how the focus on how communicative signals might emerge and how the capacity to interpret them might develop, does not yet explain what type of motivation is required to actually deal with those signals. Without the consistent production of appropriate responses to the production of communicative signals, there would be no point in producing any signal. If language is a tool to accomplish things with others, we need to understand what would lead to cooperation. The first step consists in avoiding the blind belief that all cooperation requires some prosocial attitude. A great deal of cooperation can occur while each participant in the interaction is selfishly attempting to maximize their own benefits or minimizing damaging consequences.
I describe how different types of turn-taking can be achieved via different levels of cognitive complexity and how interpretive turn-taking requires a great deal of cognitive abilities that great apes possess. Finally, I provide empirical evidence of social manipulation in non-human primates. Given our awareness of the occurrence of social manipulation during cooperation among human adults, it seems necessary to reconsider to what degree human communication and language evolution require unique prosocial motivations.
Amici, F., Aureli, F., Mundry, R., Amaro, A. S., Barroso, A. M., Ferretti, J., Call, J., (2014a). Calculated reciprocity? A comparative test with six primate species. Primates 551, 447–457.
Arbib, M. A., Ganesh, V. & Gasser, B. (2014). Dyadic Brain Modeling, Ontogenetic Ritualization of Gesture in Apes, and the Contributions of Primate Mirror Neuron Systems. Phil Trans Roy Soc B, 369 (1644), 20130414.
Blakemore, S. -J. (2012). Development of the social brain in adolescence. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 105(3), 111–116.
Bruner, J. S. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of child language, 2(1), 1–19.
Burkart, J. M., Hrdy, S. B., & Van Schaik, C. P. (2009). Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 18(5), 175–186.
Burnham, T. C., & Johnson, D. D. (2005). The biological and evolutionary logic of human cooperation. Analyse & Kritik, 27(1), 113–135.
Byrne, R. W., Cartmill, E., Genty, E., Graham, K. E., Hobaiter, C., & Tanner, J. (2017). Great ape gestures: intentional communication with a rich set of innate signals. Animal Cognition, 1–15.
Caselli, M. C., Rinaldi, P., Stefanini, S., & Volterra, V. (2012). Early action and gesture “vocabulary” and its relation with word comprehension and production. Child development, 83(2), 526–542.
Chow, C. P., Mitchell, J. F., & Miller, C. T. (2015). Vocal turn-taking in a non-human primate is learned during ontogeny. In Proc. R. Soc. B (Vol. 282, No. 1807, p. 20150069). The Royal Society.
Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2016). The Now-or-Never bottleneck: A fundamental constraint on language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 391.
Dawkins, R., & Krebs, J. R. (1978). Animal signals: information or manipulation. Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach, 21, 282–309.
Garrod, S., & Pickering, M. J. (2004). Why is conversation so easy?. Trends in cognitive sciences, 8(1), 8–11.
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and Conversation. In P. Cole & J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics. Volume 3: Speech Acts (pp. 225–242). New York: Seminar Press.
Halina, M., Rossano, F., & Tomasello, M. (2013). The ontogenetic ritualization of bonobo gestures. Animal cognition, 16(4), 653–666.
Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see. Animal Behaviour, 59(4), 771–785.
Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2001). Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know?. Animal behaviour, 61(1), 139–151.
Haun, D., & Over, H. (2015). Like me: a homophily-based account of human culture. In Epistemological dimensions of evolutionary psychology (pp. 117–130). Springer New York.
Henry, L., Craig, A. J., Lemasson, A., & Hausberger, M. (2015). Social coordination in animal vocal interactions. Is there any evidence of turn-taking? The starling as an animal model. Frontiers in psychology, 61, 1416.
Heritage, J. (1984). Garfinkel and Ethnomethodology. Cambridge: Polity Press.
Hobaiter, C., & Byrne, R. W. (2011). Serial gesturing by wild chimpanzees: its nature and function for communication. Animal cognition, 14(6), 827–838.
Hrdy, S. B. (2011). Mothers and others. Harvard University Press.
Huxley, J. (1966). The ritualization of Behaviour in animals and man. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society Vol. 2511, 7721, 249–269.
Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., and Spelke, E. S. (2007). The native language of social cognition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 1041, 12577–80.
Krebs, J. R. & Dawkins, R. (1984). Animal signals: mindreading and manipulation. In J. R. Krebs, N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural Ecology: an Evolutionary Approach (2nd edn), Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford (1984), pp. 380–402.
Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Science, 354(6308), 110–114.
Levinson, S. C. (2013). Action Formation and Ascription. In J. Sidnell & T. Stivers (Eds.), Handbook of conversation analysis (pp.103–130). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.
Levinson, S. C. (2016). Turn-taking in human communication–origins and implications for language processing. Trends in cognitive sciences, 20(1), 6–14.
Liebal, K., & Rossano, F. (2017). The give and take of food sharing in Sumatran orang-utans, Pongo abelii, and chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Animal Behaviour, 1331, 91–100.
Mascaro, O., & Sperber, D. (2009). The moral, epistemic, and mindreading components of children’s vigilance towards deception. Cognition, 112(3), 367–380.
Nielsen, M., & Blank, C. (2011). Imitation in young children: when who gets copied is more important than what gets copied. Developmental psychology, 47(4), 1050.
Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2012). Putting the social into social learning: explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children’s copying behavior. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(2), 182.
Owren, M. J., Rendall, D., & Ryan, M. J. (2010). Redefining animal signaling: influence versus information in communication. Biology & Philosophy, 25(5), 755–780.
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?The Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 11, 515–526.
Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone. University of Chicago Press.
Rossano, F. (in press). The structure and timing of human versus primate social interaction. In Hagoort, P. (ed.) Human Language: from Genes and Brains to Behavior. MIT Press.
Rossano, F., & Liebal, K. (2014). Requests’ and ‘offers’ in orangutans and human infants. Requesting in social interaction, 333–362.
Sacks, H. (1992). Lectures on Conversation: Volume II. Oxford: Blackwell.
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A Simplest Systematics for the Organization of Turn-Taking for Conversation. Language, 501, 696–735.
Schegloff, E. A. (1992). Repair after next turn: the last structurally provided for place for the defense of intersubjectivity in conversation. American Journal of Sociology, 95(5), 1295–1345.
Schegloff, E. A. (2007). Sequence Organization in Interaction: a Primer in Conversation Analysis. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2003). Signalers and receivers in animal communication. Annual review of psychology, 54(1), 145–173.
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., & Marler, P. (1980). Vervet monkey alarm calls: semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Animal Behaviour, 28(4), 1070–1094.
Shannon, C. E., & Weaver, W. (1949). The mathematical theory of communication. Urbana, Ill. Univ. Illinois Press, 11, 17.
Silk, J. B., Brosnan, S. F., Henrich, J., Lambeth, S. P., & Shapiro, S. (2013). Chimpanzees share food for many reasons: the role of kinship, reciprocity, social bonds and harassment on food transfers. Animal behaviour, 85(5), 941–947.
Smith, W. J. (1977). The behavior of communicating. Harvard University Press.
Spranger, M., & Steels, L. (2014, October). Discovering communication through ontogenetic ritualisation. In Development and Learning and Epigenetic Robotics (ICDL-Epirob), 2014 Joint IEEE International Conferences on (pp. 14–19). IEEE.
Stivers, T., Enfield, N. J., Brown, P., Englert, C., Hayashi, M., Heinemann, T., et al. (2009). Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation. PNAS, 106(26), 10587–10592.
Tinbergen, N. (1952). “Derived” activities; their causation, biological significance, origin, and emancipation during evolution. The Quarterly review of biology, 27(1), 1–32.
Tomasello, M. (2009). Why we cooperate. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Tomasello, M., Call, J., Nagell, K., Olguin, R., & Carpenter, M. (1994). The learning and use of gestural signals by young chimpanzees: A trans-generational study. Primates, 35(2), 137–154.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and brain sciences, 28(5), 673–727.
Tomasello, M., Melis, A. P., Tennie, C., Wyman, E., Herrmann, E., Gilby, I. C. & Melis, A. (2012). Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: the interdependence hypothesis. Current anthropology, 53(6).
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly review of biology, 46(1), 35–57.
Uetz, G. W., Roberts, J. A., & Taylor, P. W. (2009). Multimodal communication and mate choice in wolf spiders: female response to multimodal versus unimodal signals. Animal Behaviour, 78(2), 299–305.
Völter, C. J., Rossano, F., & Call, J. (2015). From exploitation to cooperation: social tool use in orang- utan mother–offspring dyads. Animal Behaviour, 1001, 126–134.
Völter, C. J., Rossano, F., & Call, J. (2016). Social manipulation in nonhuman primates: Cognitive and motivational determinants. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics: Control and communication in the animal and the machine (p. 194). New York: Wiley.
Wilson, M. L., Boesch, C., Fruth, B., Furuichi, T., Gilby, I. C., Hashimoto, C., Hobaiter, C. L., Hohmann, G., Itoh, N., Koops, K., et al. (2014). Lethal aggression in Pan is better explained by adaptive strategies than human impacts. Nature 5131, 414–417.
Cited by (12)
Cited by 12 other publications
Mondémé, Chloé
2023. Sequence organization in human–animal interaction. An exploration of two canonical sequences. Journal of Pragmatics 214 ► pp. 73 ff.
Abreu, Filipa & Simone Pika
2022. Turn-taking skills in mammals: A systematic review into development and acquisition. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution 10
Ball, Robbie, Sarah L. Jacobson, Matthew S. Rudolph, Miranda Trapani & Joshua M. Plotnik
2022. Acknowledging the Relevance of Elephant Sensory Perception to Human–Elephant Conflict Mitigation. Animals 12:8 ► pp. 1018 ff.
2022. Narrative pantomime: A protolanguage for persuasive communication. Lingua 271 ► pp. 103247 ff.
Hobaiter, Catherine, Kirsty E. Graham & Richard W. Byrne
2022. Are ape gestures like words? Outstanding issues in detecting similarities and differences between human language and ape gesture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 377:1860
Zhao, Minglu, Ning Tang, Annya L. Dahmani, Yixin Zhu, Federico Rossano & Tao Gao
2022. Sharing Rewards Undermines Coordinated Hunting. Journal of Computational Biology 29:9 ► pp. 1022 ff.
Due, Brian L.
2021. Distributed Perception: Co‐Operation between Sense‐Able, Actionable, and Accountable Semiotic Agents. Symbolic Interaction 44:1 ► pp. 134 ff.
Ferretti, Francesco & Ines Adornetti
2021. Persuasive conversation as a new form of communication inHomo sapiens. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 376:1824
Arbib, Michael A., Francisco Aboitiz, Judith M. Burkart, Michael C. Corballis, Gino Coudé, Erin Hecht, Katja Liebal, Masako Myowa-Yamakoshi, James Pustejovsky, Shelby S. Putt, Federico Rossano, Anne E. Russon, P. Thomas Schoenemann, Uwe Seifert, Katerina Semendeferi, Chris Sinha, Dietrich Stout, Virginia Volterra, Sławomir Wacewicz & Benjamin Wilson
Macuch Silva, Vinicius, Judith Holler, Asli Ozyurek & Seán G. Roberts
2020. Multimodality and the origin of a novel communication system in face-to-face interaction. Royal Society Open Science 7:1 ► pp. 182056 ff.
Fröhlich, Marlen, Christine Sievers, Simon W. Townsend, Thibaud Gruber & Carel P. van Schaik
2019. Multimodal communication and language origins: integrating gestures and vocalizations. Biological Reviews 94:5 ► pp. 1809 ff.
Arbib, Michael A., Francisco Aboitiz, Judith M. Burkart, Michael Corballis, Gino Coudé, Erin Hecht, Katja Liebal, Masako Myowa-Yamakoshi, James Pustejovsky, Shelby Putt, Federico Rossano, Anne E. Russon, P. Thomas Schoenemann, Uwe Seifert, Katerina Semendeferi, Chris Sinha, Dietrich Stout, Virginia Volterra, Sławomir Wacewicz & Benjamin Wilson
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 18 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.