Article published in:
Interaction and Iconicity in the Evolution of Language
Edited by Stefan Hartmann, Michael Pleyer, James Winters and Jordan Zlatev
[Interaction Studies 18:3] 2017
► pp. 465488
References

References

Aboitiz, F.
(2012) Gestures, vocalizations, and memory in language origins. Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience, 4(2), 1–15. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Andrén, M.
(2010) Children's gestures between 18 and 30 months. Lund: Media Tryck.Google Scholar
Arbib, M.
(2005) From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral and brain sciences, 28, 105–168. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012) How the brain got language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, D. F., Stokoe, W. C., & Wilcox, S. E.
(1995) Gesture and the nature of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, D. F., & Wilcox, S. E.
(2007) The gestural origin of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brown, J. E.
(2012) The evolution of symbolic communication: An embodied perspective. PhD Thesis, University of Edinburgh.
Burling, R.
(2005) The talking ape: How language evolved. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Call, J., & Tomasello, M.
(2007) The gestural communication of apes and monkeys. London: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Chandler, D.
(2007) Semiotics: The basics (2nd ed.). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Clay, Z., & Zuberbühler, K.
(2014) Vocal communication and social awareness in chimpanzees and bonobos: Insights from studies of vocal communication. In D. Dor, C. Knight, & J. Lewis (Eds.), The social origins of language (pp. 141–156). Oxford: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Collins, C.
(2013) Paleopoetics: The evolution of the literary imagination. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Corballis, M. C.
(2003) From mouth to hand: Gesture, speech, and the evolution of right-handedness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26(2), 199–208. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Donald, M.
(1991) Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of human culture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dor, D.
(2015) The instruction of imagination: Language as a social communication technology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R.
(1996) Grooming, gossip and the evolution of language. London: Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
Fay, N., Arbib, M., & Garrod, D.
(2013) How to bootstrapp a human communication system. Cognitive Science, 37, 1356–1367. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fay, N., Lister, C. J., Ellison, T. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2014) Creating a communication system from scratch: gesture beats vocalization hands down. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 354. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. T.
(2010) The evolution of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Galantucci, B., & Garrod, S.
(2010) Experimental semiotics: A new approach for studying the emergence and the evolution of human communication. Interaction Studies, 11, 1–13. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011) Experimental semiotics: A review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5(11). CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, R. A., & Gardner, B. T.
(1969) Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science 165, 664–672. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hewes, G. W.
(1973) Primate communication and the gestural origin of language. Current Anthropology, 14(1/2), 5–24. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hobaiter, C. & Byrne, R. W.
(2012) Gesture use in consortship: Wild chimpanzees’ use of gesture for an ‘evolutionarily urgent’ purpose. In S. Pika & K. Liebal (Eds.), Developments in primate gesture research (pp. 129–146). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Johansson, S.
(2005) Origins of language: Constraints on hypotheses. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kemmerer, D., Chandrasekaran, B., & Tranel, D.
(2007) A case of impaired verbalization but preserved gesticulation of motion events. Cognitive neuropsychology, 24(1), 70–114. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A.
(2004) Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012) Language and kinesic complexity. Gesture, 12, 308–326. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014a) Semiotic diversity in utterance production and the concept of “language”. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369(1651), 20130293. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014b) The 'poly-modalic' nature of utterances and its implication. In D. Dor, C. Knight, & D. Lewis (Eds.), The social origins of language (pp. 67–76). Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Kita, S., & Özyurek, A.
(2003) What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal?: Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 16–32. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Landau, B., & Gleitman, L. R.
(2009) Language and experience: Evidence from the blind child. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C., & Holler, J.
(2014) The origin of human multi-modal communication. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 369, 201303302. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lock, A., & Zukow-Goldring, P.
(2010) Preverbal communication. In J. Bremner & T. Wachs (Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of infant development (pp. 394–425). Oxford: Willey-Blackwell. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F.
(2008) The origin of speech. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D.
(1985) So you think gestures are nonverbal? Psychological Review, 92, 350–371. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005) Gesture and thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012) How language began: Gesture and speech in human evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meir, I., Aronoff, M., Börstell, C., Hwang, S., Ilkbasaran, D., Kastner, I., Lepic, R., Ben Basat, A. L., Padden, C., & Sandler, W.
(2017) The effect of being human and the basis of grammatical word order: Insights from novel communication systems and young sign languages. Cognition, 158, 189–207. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mithen, S.
(2005) The singing Neanderthals: The origins of music, language, mind and body. London: Weidenfeld and Nicholson.Google Scholar
Murillo, E., & Belinchón, M.
(2012) Gestural-vocal coordination. Gesture, 12, 16–39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paus, T.
(2001) Primate anterior cingulate cortex: Where motor control, drive and cognition interface. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(6), 417–424. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pika, S.
(2008) What is the nature of gestural communication in great apes? The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity (pp. 165–186). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schel, A. M., Townsend, S. W., Machanda, Z., Zuberbühler, K., & Slocombe, K. E.
(2013) Chimpanzee alarm call production meets key criteria for intentionality. PloS One, 8(10), e76674. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
See, A.
(2014) Reevaluating chimpanzee vocal signals: Toward a multimodal account of the origins of human communication. In M. Pina & N. Gontier (Eds.), The evolution of social communication in primates (pp. 195–215). New York: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sonesson, G.
(1997) The ecological foundations of iconicity. Semiotics around the world: Synthesis in diversity (pp. 739–742). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Stokoe, W. C.
(1991) Semantic phonology. Sign Language Studies, 71, 107–114. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Taglialatela, J. P., Russell, J. L., Schaeffer, J. A., & Hopkins, W. D.
(2011) Chimpanzee vocal signaling points to a multimodal origin of human language. PloS One, 6(4), e18852. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2008) The origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vigliocco, G., Perniss, P., & Vinson, D.
(2014) Language as a multimodal phenomenon: implications for language learning, processing and evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369(1651), 20130292. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Volterra, V., Caselli, M., Caprici, O., & Pizzuto, E.
(2005) Gesture and the emergence and development of language. In M. Tomasello & D. Slobin (Eds.), Beyond nature-nurture: Essays in honor of Elisabeth Bates (pp. 3–40). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Wacewicz, S., Zywiczynski, P., & Orzechowski, S.
(2016) Visible movements of the orofacial area. Evidence for gestural or multimodal theories of language evolution? Gesture 15(2), 251–284 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wittgenstein, L.
(1953) Philosophical investigations. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Zlatev, J.
(1997) Situated embodiment: Studies in the emergence of spatial meaning. Stockholm: Gotab.Google Scholar
(2008) From proto-mimesis to language: Evidence from primatology and social neuroscience. Journal of Physiology – Paris, 102, 137–152. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012) Cognitive semiotics: An emerging field for the transdisciplinary study of meaning. Public Journal of Semiotics, 4, 2–24.Google Scholar
(2014) Bodily mimesis and the transition to speech. In M. Pina & N. Gontier (Eds.), The evolution of social communication in primates (pp. 165–178). New York: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zywiczynski, P., & Wacewicz, S.
(2015) Ewolucja języka. W stronę hipotez gesturalnych. Toruń: Wydawnictwo Naukowe Uniwersytetu Mikołaja Kopernika.Google Scholar
Zywiczynski, P., Wacewicz, S., & Sibierska, M.
(2016) Defining pantomime for language evolution research Topoi. An International Review of Philosophy [in press]. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 8 other publications

Adornetti, Ines, Francesco Ferretti, Alessandra Chiera, Slawomir Wacewicz, Przemysław Żywiczyński, Valentina Deriu, Andrea Marini, Rita Magni, Laura Casula, Stefano Vicari & Giovanni Valeri
2019. Do Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders Understand Pantomimic Events?. Frontiers in Psychology 10 Crossref logo
Brown, Steven, Emma Mittermaier, Tanishka Kher & Paul Arnold
2019. How Pantomime Works: Implications for Theories of Language Origin. Frontiers in Communication 4 Crossref logo
Ferretti, Francesco & Ines Adornetti
2021.  Persuasive conversation as a new form of communication in Homo sapiens . Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 376:1824 Crossref logo
Macuch Silva, Vinicius, Judith Holler, Asli Ozyurek & Seán G. Roberts
2020. Multimodality and the origin of a novel communication system in face-to-face interaction. Royal Society Open Science 7:1  pp. 182056 ff. Crossref logo
Zywiczynski, Przemyslaw, Nathalie Gontier & Slawomir Wacewicz
2017. The evolution of (proto-)language: Focus on mechanisms. Language Sciences 63  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
Żywiczyński, Przemysław
2019. How research on language evolution contributes to linguistics. Yearbook of the Poznan Linguistic Meeting 5:1  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
Żywiczyński, Przemysław, Marta Sibierska, Sławomir Wacewicz, Joost van de Weijer, Francesco Ferretti, Ines Adornetti, Alessandra Chiera & Valentina Deriu
2021. Evolution of conventional communication. A cross-cultural study of pantomimic re-enactments of transitive events. Language & Communication 80  pp. 191 ff. Crossref logo
Żywiczyński, Przemysław, Sławomir Wacewicz & Casey Lister
2021. Pantomimic fossils in modern human communication. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 376:1824 Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 14 september 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.

video