Article published In:
Interaction Studies
Vol. 22:1 (2021) ► pp.2454
References
Altmann, S. A.
(1974) Baboons, space, time and energy. American Zoologist, 141, 221–248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, J. D., & Baldwin, J. I.
(1972) The ecology and behavior of squirrel monkeys (Saimiri oerstedii) in a natural forest in western Panama. Folia Primatologica, 181, 161–184. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barton RA
(1993) Sociospatial mechanisms of feeding competition in female olive baboons, Papio anubis. Animal Behaviour, 461, 791–802. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Christman, M. C., & Lewis, D.
(2005) Spatial distribution of dominant animals within a group: comparison of four statistical tests of location. Animal Behaviour, 701, 73–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, P. J., & Evans, F. C.
(1954) Distance to nearest neighbor as a measure of spatial relationships in populations. Ecology, 35(4), 445–453. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clutton-Brock, T. H.
(1974) Primate social organization and ecology. Nature, 2501, 539–542. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Collins, D. A.
(1984) Factors affecting mating success of male yellow baboons in Ruaha-National-Park, Tanzania. International Journal of Primatology, 51, 329–329.Google Scholar
Corradino, C.
(1990) Proximity structure in a captive colony of Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata fuscata): An application of multidimensional scaling. Primates, 31(3), 351–362. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
David, H. A.
(1987) Ranking from unbalanced paired-comparison data. Biometrika, 741, 432–436. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ehardt-Seward, C., & Bramblett, C. A.
(1980) The structure of social space among a captive group of vervet monkeys. Folia Primatologica, 34(3–4), 214–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flack, J. C., & de Waal, F. B. M.
(2004) Dominance style, social power, and conflict management: a conceptual framework. In Thierry, B.; Singh, M. & Kaumanns, W. (Eds.), Macaque societies: a model for the study of social organization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D.
(1971) Geometry for the selfish herd. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 311, 295–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hemelrijk, C. K.
(1998) Spatial centrality of dominants without positional preference. In C. Adami (Eds.), et al. Artificial life VI. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2000) Towards the integration of social dominance and spatial structure. Animal Behaviour, 591, 1035–1048. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hirsch, B. T.
(2007) Costs and benefits of within-group spatial position: a feeding competition model. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 82(1), 9–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krause, J.
(1993) The effect of Schreckstoff on the shoaling behavior of the minnow – a test of Hamilton’s selfish herd theory. Animal Behaviour, 451, 1019–1024. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1994) Differential fitness returns in relation to spatial position in groups. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society, 691, 187–206. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C., & Amsler, S. J.
(2003) Social and spatial aspects of male subgrouping in a community of wild chimpanzees. Behaviour, 140(7), 869–884. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Murton, R. K., Isaacson, A. J., & Westwood, N. J.
(1971) The significance of gregarious feeding behaviour and adrenal stress in a population of Wood-pigeons Columba palumbus . Journal of Zoology, 165(1), 53–84. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Romey, W. L.
(1997) Inside or outside? Testing evolutionary predictions of positional effects. In J. K. Parrish & W. M. Hamner (Eds.), Animals Groups in Three Dimensions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ron, T., Henzi, S. P., & Motro, U.
(1996) Do female chacma baboons compete for a safe spatial position in a southern woodland habitat? Behaviour, 1331, 475–490. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ross, R. A., & Giller, P. S.
(1988) Observations on the activity patterns and social interactions of a captive group of blackcapped or brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Primates, 29(3), 307–317. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ross, S. R., Calcutt, S., Schapiro, S. J., & Hau, J.
(2011) Space use selectivity by chimpanzees and gorillas in an indoor–outdoor enclosure. American journal of primatology, 73(2), 197–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schreier, A. L., & Swedell, L.
(2009) The fourth level of social structure in a multi-level society: ecological and social functions of clans in hamadryas baboons. American Journal of Primatology: Official Journal of the American Society of Primatologists, 71(11), 948–955. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sigg, H.
(1980) Differentiation of female positions in hamadryas one male units. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 531, 265–302. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thierry, B.
(2000) Covariation of conflict management patterns across macaque species. In Aureli, F. & de Waal, F. B. M. (Eds.), Natural conflict resolution. California: University of California Press.Google Scholar
(2004) Social epigenesis. In Thierry, B.; Singh, M. & Kaumanns, W. (Eds.), Macaque societies: a model for the study of social organization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(2007) Unity in diversity: lessons from macaque societies. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 16(6), 224–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tinbergen, N., Impekoven, M., & Franck, D.
(1967) An experiment on spacing-out as a defence against predation. Behaviour, 281, 307–321. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Schaik, C. P.
(1983) Why are diurnal primates living in groups? Behaviour, 871, 120–144. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wang, E., & Milton, K.
(2003) Intragroup social relationships of male Alouatta palliata on Barro Colorado Island, Republic of Panama. International Journal of Primatology, 24(6), 1227–1243. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zhang, P., Li, B. G., Qi, X. G., MacIntosh, A. J., & Watanabe, K.
(2012) A proximity-based social network of a group of Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana). International Journal of Primatology, 33(5), 1081–1095. DOI logoGoogle Scholar