Article published In:
Interaction Studies
Vol. 24:3 (2023) ► pp.464483
References (24)
References
Barr, R., & Hayne, H. (2003). It’s not what you know, it’s who you know: Older siblings facilitate imitation during infancy. International Journal of Early Years Education, 11 (1), 117–121. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bekkering, H., Wohlschlager, A., & Gattis, M. (2000). Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53 (1), 153–164. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brooker, I., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2013). Is a bird an apple? The effect of speaker labeling accuracy on infants’ word learning, imitation, and helping behaviors. Infancy, 18 (s1), E46–E68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brosseau-Liard, P. E., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2014). Sensitivity to confidence cues increases during the second year of life. Infancy, 19 (5), 461–475. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76 (6), 893–910. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2006). Social learning and social cognition: The case for pedagogy. In Y. Munakata & M. H. Johnson (Eds.), Processes of change in brain and cognitive development. Attention and Performance, XXI1 (pp. 249–274). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Floor, P., & Akhtar, N. (2006). Can 18-month-old infants learn words by listening in on conversations? Infancy, 9 (3), 327–339. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haun, D. B. M., Rekers, Y., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Majority-biased transmission in chimpanzees and human children, but not orangutans. Current Biology, 22 (8), 727–731. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krogh-Jespersen, S., & Echols, C. H. (2012). The influence of speaker reliability on first versus second label learning. Child Development, 83 (2), 581–590. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matheson, H., Moore, C., & Akhtar, N. (2013). The development of social learning in interactive and observational contexts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114 (2), 161–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (1988). Infant imitation after a 1-week delay: Long-term memory for novel acts and multiple stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 24 (4), 470–476. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1999). Born to learn: What infants learn from watching us. In N. A. Fox, L. A. Leavitt & J. G. Warhol (Eds.), The role of early experience in infant development (pp. 145–164). Skillman, NJ: Pediatric Institute Publications.Google Scholar
Moraru, C-A., Gomez, J-C., & McGuigan, N. (2016). Developmental changes in the influence of conventional and instrumental cues on over-imitation in 3- to 6-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 145 1, 34–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nielsen, M. (2006). Copying actions and copying outcomes: Social learning through the second year. Developmental Psychology, 42 (3), 555–565. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nielsen, M., Moore, C., & Mohamedally, J. (2012). Young children overimitate in third-party contexts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112 (1), 73–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Poulin-Dubois, D., Brooker, I., & Polonia, A. (2011). Infants prefer to imitate a reliable person. Infant Behavior and Development, 34 (2), 303–309. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ray, E., & Heyes, C. (2011). Imitation in infancy: The wealth of the stimulus. Developmental Science, 14 (1), 92–105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Repacholi, B. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). Emotional eavesdropping: Infants selectively respond to indirect emotional signals. Child Development, 78 (2), 503–521. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shimpi, P. M., Akhtar, N., & Moore, C. (2013). Toddlers’ imitative learning in interactive and observational contexts: The role of age and familiarity of the model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116 (2), 309–323. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shneidman, L., Todd, R., & Woodward, A. L. (2014). Why do child-directed interactions support imitative learning in young children? PLoS ONE, 9 (10), e110891. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stenberg, G. (2019). The situational context and the reliability of an adult model influence infants’ imitation. Interaction Studies, 20 (2), 375–390. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020). Infant imitation in a third-party context. Interaction Studies, 21 (3), 387–411. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yang, D., Sidman, J., & Bushnell, E. W. (2010). Beyond the information given: Infants’ transfer of actions learned through imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106 (1), 62–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zmyj, N., Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., & Daum, M. M. (2010). The reliability of a model influences 14-month-olds’ imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106 (4), 208–220. DOI logoGoogle Scholar