References (102)
References
Abelson, R. (1985). A variance explanation paradox: When a little is a lot. Psychological Bulletin, 97(1), 129–133. <[URL]>
Aloe, A., & Becker, B. (2012). An effect size for regression predictors in meta-analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 371, 278–297. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anderson, M. (2009). Retrieval. In Baddeley, Eysenck, & Anderson (Eds.), 161–189.Google Scholar
Anderson, S., & Maxwell, S. (2016). There’s more than one way to conduct a replication study: Beyond statistical significance. Psychological Methods, 21(1), 1–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Asher, J. (1969). The total physical response approach to second language learning. Modern Language Journal, 53(1), 3–17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A., Eysenck, M., & Anderson, M. (Eds.). (2009). Memory. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Barcroft, J. (2015). Lexical input processing and vocabulary learning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., D’Amico, S., Jacobsen, T., Székely, A., Andonova, E., Devescovi, A., Herron, D., & Tzeng, O. (2003). Timed picture naming in seven languages. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 10(2), 344–380. [URL]
Begg, I. (1972). Recall of meaningful phrases. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11(4), 431–439. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boers, F., Dang, C. T., & Strong, B. (2017). Comparing the effectiveness of phrase-focused exercises. A partial replication of Boers, Demecheleer, Coxhead, and Webb (2014). Language Teaching Research, 21(3), 362–280. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boers, F., Demecheleer, M., Coxhead, A., & Webb, S. (2014). Gauging the effects of exercises on verb-noun collocations. Language Teaching Research, 18(1), 54–74. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boers, F., Eyckmans, J., & Lindstromberg, S. (2014). The effect of a discrimination task on L2 learners’ recall of collocations and compounds. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 24(3), 357–369. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boers, F., & Lindstromberg, S. (2009). Optimizing a Lexical Approach to Instructed Second Language Acquisition. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave-Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012). Experimental and intervention studies of formulaic sequences in a second language. Annual Rev. of Applied Linguistics, 321, 83–110. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boers, F., Lindstromberg, S., & Eyckmans, J. (2012). Are alliterative word combinations comparatively easy to remember for adult learners? RELC Journal, 43(1), 127–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014a). Some explanations for the slow acquisition of L2 collocations. Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 111, 41–62. <[URL]>
(2014b). When does assonance make lexical phrases memorable? European Journal of Applied Linguistics and TEFL, 3(1), 93–107. <[URL]>
(2014c). Is alliteration mnemonic without awareness-raising? Language Awareness, 23(4), 291–303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley & Sons. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Braver, S., Theommes, F., & Rosenthal, R. (2014). Continuously accumulating meta-analysis and replicability. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 91, 333–342. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1976). A first language: The early stages. Harmondsworth, MA: Penguin.Google Scholar
Brysbaert, M., Warriner, A., & Kuperman, V. (2014). Concreteness ratings for 40,000 generally known English word lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 46(3), 904–911. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cohen, R. (1989). Memory for action events: The power of enactment. Educational Psychology Review, 1(1), 57–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Conneely, K., & Boehnke, M. (2007). So many correlated tests, so little time! Rapid adjustment of P values for multiple correlated tests. American Journal of Human Genetics, 81(6), 1158–1168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cop, U., Keuleers, E., Drieghe, D., & Duyck, W. (2015). Frequency effects in monolingual and biliingual natural reading. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 22(5), 1216–1234. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Criss, A., Aue, W., & Smith, L. (2011). The effects of word frequency and context variability in cued recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 64(2), 119–132. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crossley, S., Salsbury, T., Titak, A., & McNamara, D. (2014). Frequency effects in second language acquisition: Word types, word tokens, and word production. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 19(3), 301–332. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cumming, G. (2012). Understanding the new statistics: Effect sizes, confidence intervals, and meta-analysis. Hove: Routledge.Google Scholar
(2014). The new statistics: Why and how. Psychological Science, 251, 7–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davies, M. (2008–2018). The Corpus of Contemporary American English (COCA). <[URL]>
De Groot, A., & Keijzer, R. (2000). What is hard to learn is easy to forget: The roles of words concreteness, cognate status, and word frequency in foreign-language vocabulary learning and forgetting. Language Learning, 50(1), 1–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dell, G., & J. Gordon. (2003). Neighbors in the lexicon: Friends or foes? In N. Schiller & A. Meyer (Eds.), Phonetics and phonology in language comprehension and production: Differences and similarities (pp. 9–37). Berlin: Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dellantonio, S., Mulatti, C., Pastore, L., & Job, R. (2014). Measuring inconsistencies can lead you forward: Imageability and the x-ception theory. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 708. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeLosh, E., & McDaniel, M. (1996). The role of order information in free recall: Application to the word-frequency effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22(5), 1136–1146. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Del Re, A. (2014). compute.es, version 0.2–4. (R package). <[URL]>
Dewhurst, S., Brandt, K., & Sharp, M. (2004). Intention to learn influences the word frequency effect in recall but not in recognition memory. Memory and Cognition, 32(8), 1316–1325. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diana, R., & Reder, L. (2006). The low-frequency encoding disadvantage: Word frequency affects processing demands. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(4), 805–815. <[URL]>
Durrant, P., & Doherty, A. (2010). Are high-frequency collocations psychologically real? Investigating the thesis of collocational priming. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 6(2),125–155. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Durrant, P. (2014). Corpus frequency and second language learners’ knowledge of collocations: A meta-analysis. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 19(4), 443–477. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. (2002). Frequency effects in language processing: A review with implications for theories of implicit and explicit language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24(2), 143–188. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N., & Sagarra, N. (2011). Learned attention in adult language acquisition: A replication and generalization study and meta-analysis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 331, 589–624. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, P. (2010). Essential guide to effect sizes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eyckmans, J., Boers, F., & Stengers, H. (2007). Identifying chunks: Who can see the wood for the trees? Language Forum, 331, 85–100.Google Scholar
Eyckmans, J., & Lindstromberg, S. (2016). The power of sound in L2 idiom learning. Language Teaching Research, 21(3), 341–365. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, M., & Eysenck, C. (1980). Effects of processing depth, distinctiveness, and word frequency on retention. British Journal of Psychology, 71(2), 263–274. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gardner, M., Rothkopf, E., Lapan, R., & Lafferty, T. (1987). The word frequency effect in lexical decision: Finding a frequency-based component. Memory and Cognition 15(1), 24–28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glanzer, M., & Ehrenreich, S. (1979). Structure and search of the internal lexicon. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18(4), 381–398. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. (2006). Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldstein, R., & Vitevitch, M. (2014). The influence of clustering coefficient on word-learning: How groups of similar sounding words facilitate acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gordon, B., & Caramazza, A. (1982). Lexical decision for open- and closed-class words: Failure to replicate differential frequency sensitivity. Brain and Language, 15(1), 143–160. <[URL]>
Guolo, A., & Varin, C. (2017). Random-effects meta-analysis: The number of studies matters. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 26(3), 1500–1518. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hall, J. (1954). Learning as a function of word-frequency. American Journal of Psychology, 67(1), 138–140. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamrick, P., & Rebuschat, P. (2014). Frequency effects, learning conditions, and the development of implicit and explicit lexical knowledge. In J. Connor-Linton, & L. Amoroso (Eds.), Measured language: Quantitative approaches to acquisition, assessment, processing and variation (pp. 125–139). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Han, M., Storkel, H., Lee, J., & Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (2015). The influence of word characteristics on the vocabulary of children with cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 20(3), 242–251. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Henriksen, B. (2012). Research on L2 learners’ collocational competence and development. In C. Bardel, C. Lindqvist, & B. Laufer (Eds.), EUROSLA Yearbook 5: L2 vocabulary acquisition: Knowledge and use, (pp. 29–56). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hulstijn, J. (2001). Deliberate and incidental second language vocabulary learning: A reappraisal of elaboration, rehearsal and automaticity. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 258–286). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Izura, C., & Ellis, A. (2002). Age of acquisition effects in word recognition and production in first and second languages. Psicológica, 231, 245–281. <[URL]>
Jenkins, J., Stein, M., & Wysocki, K. (1984). Learning vocabulary through reading. American Educational Research Journal, 21(4), 767–787. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Judd, C., Westfall, J., & Kenny, D. (2016). Experiments with more than one random factor: Designs, analytic models, and statistical power. Annual Review of Psychology, 681, 17.1–17.25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuperman, V., Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). Age-of-acquisition ratings for 30,000 English words. Behavioral Research Methods, 44(4),978–990. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Laufer, B. (2005). Focus on Form in second language vocabulary acquisition. EUROSLA Yearbook 5(1), 223–250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Laufer, B., & Waldman, T. (2011). Verb-noun collocations in second language writing: A corpus analysis of learners’ English. Language Learning, 61(2), 647–672. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lindstromberg, S. (2016). Inferential statistics in Language Teaching Research: A review and ways forward. Language Teaching Research, 20(6), 741–768. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lindstromberg, S., & Eyckmans, J. How big is the positive effect of assonance on the near-term recall of L2 collocations? ITL International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 165(1), 19–45. DOI logo
Lindstromberg, S., & Boers, F. (2008). The mnemonic effect of noticing alliteration in lexical chunks. Applied Linguistics, 29(2), 200–222. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lindstromberg, S., & Eyckmans, J. (2017). The particular need for replication in the quantitative study of SLA: A case study of the mnemonic effect of assonance in collocations. Journal of the European Second Language Association, 1(1), 126–136. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Loftus, G., & Loftus, E. (1974). The influence of one memory retrieval on a subsequent memory retrieval. Memory and Cognition, 2(3), 467–471. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lohnas, L., & Kahana, M. (2013). Parametric effects of word frequency in memory for mixed frequency lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(6), 1943–1946. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lotto, L., & de Groot, A. (1998). Effects of learning method and word type on acquiring vocabulary in an unfamiliar language. Language Learning, 48(1), 31–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martinez, R., & Murphy, V. (2011). Effect of frequency and idiomaticity on second language reading comprehension. TESOL Quarterly, 45(2), 267–290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McChesney, J. (2016). You should summarize data with the geometric mean. <[URL]>
McLeod, C., & Kampe, K. (1996). Word frequency effects in recall, recognition, and word fragment completion tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(1), 132–142. <[URL]>
Meschyan, G., & Hernandez, A. (2002). Age of acquisition and word frequency: Determinants of object-naming speed and accuracy. Memory & Cognition, 30(2), 262–269. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moon, R. (1998). Fixed expressions and idioms in English: A corpus-based approach. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Mullen, B., Muellerleihe, P., & Bryant, B. (2001). Cumulative meta-analysis: A consideration of indicators of sufficiency and stability. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(11), 1450–1462. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nation, P. (2013). Learning Vocabulary in Another Language, 3rd edn. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oldfield, R. C., & Wingfield, A. (1965). Response latencies in naming objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17(4), 273–281. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oxford University Press. (2017). How to use the OED key to frequency. <[URL]>
Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual-coding approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Paivio, A., & Desrochers, A. (1979). Effects of an imagery mnemonic on second language recall and comprehension. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 33(1), 17–28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. Vehoeven, C. Elbro, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy, (pp. 189–213). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peters, E. (2014). The effects of repetition and time of post-test administration on EFL learners’ form recall of single words and collocations. Language Teaching Research, 18(1), 75–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016). The learning burden of collocations: The role of interlexical and intralexical factors. Language Teaching Research, 20(1), 113–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pisoni, D., Nusbaum, H., Luce, P., & Slowiaczek, L. (1985). Speech Communication, 4(1–3), 75–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. (Version 3.3.1). Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. <[URL]>
Reilly, J., & Kean, J. (2007). Formal distinctiveness of high- and low-imageability nouns: Analyses and theoretical implications. Cognitive Science, 31(1), 157–168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, S. (2011). Memory for emotional words in sentences: The importance of emotional contrast. Cognition and Emotion, 26(6), 1015–1035. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmitt, N. (2008). Review article: Instructed second language vocabulary learning. Language Teaching Research, 121, 329–363. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Siyanova-Chanturia, A., & Janssen, N. (2018). Production of familiar phrases: Frequency effects in native speakers and second language learners. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. Advance online publication. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sonbul, S., & Schmitt, N. (2013). Explicit and implicit lexical knowledge: Acquisition of collocations under different input conditions. Language Learning, 63(1), 121–159. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stadthagen-Gonzales, H., & Davis, C. (2006). The Bristol norms for age of acquisition, imageability, and familiarity. Behavioral Research Methods, 38(4), 598–605. <[URL]>
Storkel, H., & Morrisette, M. (2002). The lexicon and phonology: Interactions in language acquisition. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 33(1), 24–37. <[URL]>
Tellings, A., Coppens, K., Gelissen, J., & Schreuder, R. (2013). Clusters of word properties as predictors of elementary school children’s performance on two word tasks. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34(3), 461–481. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
VanArsdall, J., Nairne, J., Pandeira, J., & Blunt, J. (2013). Adaptive memory: Animacy processing produces mnemonic advantages. Experimental Psychology, 60(3), 172–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Software, 36(3), 1–48. <[URL]>
Waring, R., & Takaki, M. (2003). At what rate do learners learn and retain new vocabulary from reading a graded reader? Reading in a Foreign Language, 15(2), 130–163. <[URL]>
Watkins, M., LeCompte, D., & Kim, K. (2000). Role of study strategy in recall of mixed lists of common and rare words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(1), 239–245. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Webb, S., Newton, J., & Chang, A. (2013). Incidental learning of collocation. Language Learning, 63(1), 91–120. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Westfall, J., Kenny, D., & Judd, C. (2014). Statistical power and optimal design in experiments in which samples of participants respond to samples of stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1431, 220–245. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whaley, C. (1978). Word-nonword classification time. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17(2), 143–154. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (2)

Cited by two other publications

Yi, Wei & Yanlu Zhong
2024. The processing advantage of multiword sequences: A meta-analysis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 46:2  pp. 427 ff. DOI logo
Lindstromberg, Seth & June Eyckmans
2021. THE RETRIEVABILITY OF L2 ENGLISH MULTIWORD ITEMS IN A CONTEXT OF STRONGLY FORM-FOCUSED EXPOSURE. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 43:5  pp. 1040 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 13 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.