Article published In:
Papers of the workshop on Language and Prosody
Edited by Karel van den Eynde and Nicole Delbecque
[ITL - International Journal of Applied Linguistics 60/61] 1983
► pp. 126
References

Bibliographie

ABBS, M.S. & MINIFIE, F.D.
(1969) : Effects of acoustic cues in fricatives on perceptual confusion in preschool childres, JASA 46 (6), 1535-42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
BAILEY, P.J. & SUMMERFIELD, Q.
(1978) : Some observations on te perception of [s]+ stop culsters, Haskins SR-53 (2), 25-60.Google Scholar
(1980) : Information in speech: observations on the perception of [s ] - stop clusters, J.E.P. (Human Perception & Performance) 61, 536-63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
von BEKESY, G.
(1963) : Hearing theories and complex sounds, JASA 35 (4), 588-601. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van den BROECKE, M.P.R. & van HEUVEN, V.J.J.P.
(1979) : One or two velar fricatives in Dutch. in : Anniversaries in Phonetics, Studia Gratulatoria dedicated to Hendrik Mol, 51-67.Google Scholar
CARDEN, G.; LEVITT, A.G.; JUSCZYK, P.W. & WALLEY, A.
(1981) : Evidence for phonetic processing of cues to place of articulation : perceived manner effects, perceived place, P & P 291, 26-36.Google Scholar
CARNEY, P.J. & MOLL, K.L.
(1971) : A cine fluorographic investigation of fricative consonant - vowel coarticulation, Phonetica 231,193-202. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DAELEMAN, J.
(1980) : Durée consonantique en tetela, article non publié.Google Scholar
DELATTRE, P.C.; LIBERMAN, A.M. & COOPER, F.S.
(1962) : Formant transitions and loci as acoustic correlates of place of articulation in American fricatives, Studia Linguistica 161, 104-21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DUBOIS, S.; LANDERCY, A. & RENARD, R.
(1977) : Perception du timbre des consonnes françaises orales. Activités, Conférences et Travaux (ACT) 21, 35-60, (Université de l’Etat à Mons.)Google Scholar
FUJISAKI, H. & KUNISAKI, O.
(1978) : Analysis, recognition, and perception of voiceless fricative consonants in Japanese, IEEE-Transactions (ASSP) 261, 21-7.Google Scholar
GLAVE, R.D.
(1972) Zur akustischen Struktur stimmloser Frikativlaute, PICPS 71, 493-8.Google Scholar
GREEN, P.S.
(1958) : Consonant - vowel transitions : a spectrographic study, Studia Linguistica 121, 57-105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
HARRIS, K.S.
(1954) : Cues for the identification of fricatives in American English, JASA 261, 952 (Abstract). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1956) : Some acoustic cues for the fricative consonants, JASA 281, 160 (Abstract). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1958) Cues für the discrimination of American English fricatives in spoken syllables, L & S 11, 1-7.Google Scholar
HEINZ, J.M.
(1960) : Spectrum matching of fricative consonants, JASA 321, 1517 (Abstract) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
HEINZ, J.M. & STEVENS, K.N.
(1961) :On bhe properties of voiceless fricative consonants, JASA 331, 589-96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
HOUTGAST, T.
(1972) : Psychophysical evidence for lateral inhibition in hearing, JASA 51 (6), 1885-94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
HUGHES, G.W. & HALLE, M.
(1956) : Spectral properties of fricative consonants, JASA 281, 303-10. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
JASSEM, W.
(1962) : The acoustics of consonants, PICPS 41, 50-72.Google Scholar
(1965) : The formants of fricative consonants, L & S 8 (1), 1-16.Google Scholar
KUNISAKI, O. & FUJISAKI, H.
(1977) : On the influence of context upon perception of voiceless fricative consonants. Annual Bulletin of the Research Institute of Logopedics and Phoniatrics 111, 85-91.Google Scholar
LACERDA, F.P.
(1982) : Acoustic perceptual study of the Portugese voiceless fricatives, J.Ph. 10 (1), 11-22.Google Scholar
LaRIVIERE, C.J.; WINITZ, H. & HERRIMAN, E.
(1975) : The distribution of perceptual cues in English prevocalic fricatives, J.S.H.R. 181, 613-22.Google Scholar
MALECOT, A. & CHERMAK, A.
(1966) : Place cues for /p,t,k/ in lower cutoff frequency shifts of contiguous /s/. L & S 91, 162-9.Google Scholar
MANN, V.A. & REPP, B.H.
(1979) : Influence of vocalic context on perception of the [∫] - [s] distinction: I. Temporal factors, Haskins SR-59/601, 49-64.Google Scholar
(1980) : Influence of vocalic context on perception of the [∫] - [s] distinction: III. Acoustic observations, Haskins SR-621, 177-85.Google Scholar
(1980b) : Influence of vocalic context on perception of the [∫] - [s] distinction, P & P 281, 213-28.Google Scholar
(1981) : Influence of preceding fricative on stop consonant perception, JASA 691, 548-58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MARTONY, J.
(1962a) : On the synthesis and perception of voiceless Swedish fricatives, Stockholm Q.E.S.R. 1962/1, 17-22.Google Scholar
(1962b) : On the perception of Swedish voiceless fricatives, Stockholm Q.P.S.R 1962/2, 25-28.Google Scholar
MARTONY, J.; CEDERLUND, C.; LILJENCRANTS, J. & LINDBLOM, B.
(1962) : On the analysis and synthesis of vowels and fricatives, PICPS 41, 208-13.Google Scholar
McCASLAND, G.P.
(1978) : Formant transition place cues of intervocalic fricatives, JASA 631 (Suppl. 1), S21 (Abstract). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
PLOMP, R.
(1976) : Aspects of Tone Sensation : A Psychophysical Study, London - N.Y. : Academic Press.Google Scholar
RAINBOLT, H.R. & SCHUBERT, E.D.
(1968) : Use of noise bands to establish noise pitch, JASA 431, 316-23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
REPP, B.H.
(1980) : Phonetic trading relations and context effects : new experimental evidence for a speech mode of perception. Haskins SR-67/681, 1-40.Google Scholar
REPP, B.H.; L’JBERMAN, A.M.; ECCARDT, T. & PESETSKY, D.
(1978) : Perceptual integration of cues for stop, fricative and affricate manner, Haskins SR-53 (2), 61-84.Google Scholar
REPP, B.H.; LIBERMAN, A.M.; ECCARDT, T. & PESETSKY, D.
(1978b) : Perceptual integration of acoustic cues for stop,fricative, and affricate manner, J.E.P. (Human Perception & Performance) 41, 621-37. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
REPP, B.H. & MANN, V.A.
(1980) : Influence of vocalic context on perception of the [∫] - [s] distinction: II. Spectral factors, Haskins SR-611, 65-84.Google Scholar
(1981a) : Perceptual assessment of fricative - stop articulation, JASA 691, 1154-63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1981b) : Fricative - stop coarticulation : acoustic and perceptual evidence, Haskins SR-67/681, 255-66.Google Scholar
SCHWARTZ, M.F.
(1967) : Transitions in American English [s] as cues to the identity of adjacent stop consonants, JASA 421, 897-9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
SCULLY, C.
(1971) : A comparison of /s/ and /z/ for an English speaker, L & S 14 (2), 187-200.Google Scholar
SHARF, D.J.
(1968) : Distinctiveness od “defective” fricative sounds”, L & S 111, 38-45.Google Scholar
(1971) : Perceptual parameters of consonant sounds, L & S 14 (2), 169-77.Google Scholar
SHARF, D. & HEMEYER, J.
(1972) : Identification of place of consonant articulation from vowel formant transitions, JASA 51 (2), 652-58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
SMALL, A.M. & DANILOFF, R.G.
(1967) : Pitch of noise bands, JASA 41 (2), 506-12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
SOLI, S.D.
(1981) Second formant in fricatives : acoustic consequences of fricative - vowel coarticulation, JASA 70 (4), 976-84. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
STEVENS, K.
(1971) : Airflow and turbulent noise for fricative and stop consonants : static considerations, JASA 501, 1180-92. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
STEVENS, K.N. & BLUMSTEIN, S.E.
(1981) : The search for invariant acoustic correlates of phonetic features, in : EIMAS, P.D. & MILLER, J.L. (eds.) (1981) : Perspectives on the Study of Speech, 1-38.Google Scholar
STREVENS, P.
(1960) : Spectra of fricative noise in human speech, L & S 31, 32-49.Google Scholar
SUMMERFIELD, Q.; BAILEY, P.J.; SETON, J. & DORMAN, M.F.
(1981) : Fricative envelope parameters and silent intervals in distinguishing “slit ” and “split”, Phonetica 38 (1-3), 181-92. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
TARNOCZY, T.
(1954) : Die akustische Struktur der stimmlosen Engelaute, Acta linguistica 41, 313-49 (Budapest).Google Scholar
ULDALL, E.
(1964) : Transitions in fricative noise, L & S 71, 13-5.Google Scholar
WEISMER, G.
(1980) : Control of voicing distinction for intervocalic stops and fricatives : some data and theoretical considerations, J.Ph. 8 (4), 427-38.Google Scholar
WHALEN, D.H.
(1981) Effects of vocalic formant transitions and vowel quality on the English [s] - [š] boundary, JASA 69 (1), 275-82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
YENI-KOMSHIAN, G.H. & SOLI, S.D.
(1981) : Recognition of vowels from information in fricatives: perceptual evidence of fricative vowel coarticulation, JASA 70 (4), 966-75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar