Fluctuating cognitive benefits in children attending early bilingual immersive instruction
Cognitive benefits have been reported in early bilinguals as well as in children attending a Content and Language
Integrated Learning (CLIL) school programme. Recent findings, however, show divergent results. These inconsistent findings seem to
be attributable, among other factors, to the divergent tasks applied. This study aimed to determine whether a two-year CLIL
programme has positive effects by applying the same tasks for which advantages were shown previously and to examine whether
potential gains might impact children’s academic performance. Thirty 7-year-old French-speaking pupils who had been enrolled in a
CLIL programme since the age of 5 and 30 French-speaking monolinguals following a traditional school programme were compared on
different attentional and executive tasks after being matched on potential confounding variables. No attentional and executive
differences were observed, but a significant effect was observed in favour of monolinguals on a task assessing mathematical
skills. The implications of these results are discussed.
Des bénéfices cognitifs ont été rapportés chez des bilingues précoces ainsi que chez des enfants suivant un
programme scolaire d’enseignement d’une matière par l’intégration d’une langue étrangère (EMILE). Cependant, des résultats récents
montrent des divergences. Ces résultats incohérents semblent être attribuables, entre autres facteurs, aux différentes tâches
appliquées. Cette étude visait à déterminer si un programme EMILE de deux ans a des effets positifs en appliquant les mêmes tâches
pour lesquelles des avantages ont été démontrés précédemment et à examiner si les gains potentiels peuvent avoir un impact sur les
performances scolaires des enfants. Trente élèves francophones de 7 ans inscrits dans un programme EMILE depuis l’âge de 5 ans et
30 monolingues francophones suivant un programme traditionnel ont été comparés sur différentes tâches attentionnelles et
exécutives après avoir été appariés sur plusieurs variables confondantes potentielles. Aucune différence attentionnelle et
exécutive n’a été observée, mais un effet significatif a été observé en faveur des monolingues dans une tâche évaluant les
compétences mathématiques. Les implications de ces résultats sont discutées.
Article outline
- 1.Introduction
- 1.1Attentional and executive advantages of bilingualism
- 1.2Attentional and executive advantages in the context of CLIL education
- 1.3Inconsistence of data regarding the cognitive effects of CLIL education
- 1.4Factors responsible for data inconsistency
- 1.5The present study
- 2.Method
- 2.1Participants
- 2.2Materials and procedures
- 2.2.1Preliminary measures – Background questionnaire
- 2.2.2Attentional and executive tasks
- 2.2.3Academic measures
- 2.2.4Ethics statement
- 2.2.5General procedure
- 2.2.6Statistical analyses
- 3.Results
- 3.1Attentional and executive measures
- 3.2Academic measures
- 4.Discussion
- 5.Conclusions
- Competing interests
-
References
References (71)
References
Adam, S., Van der Linden, M., & Collette, F. (2002). L’attention
dans le vieillissement normal. In J. Couillet, M. Leclercq, C. Moroni, & P. Azouvi (Eds.), La
neuropsychologie de
l’attention (pp. 129–155). Marseille: Solal.
Baggetta, P. &, Alexander, P. A. (2016). Conceptualization
and Operationalization of Executive Function. Mind, Brain and
Education,
10
(1), 10–33.
Baker, C. (2011). Foundations
of bilingual education and bilingualism (5th
Edition). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Barac, R., & Bialystok, E. (2011). Cognitive
development of bilingual children. Language
Teaching,
44
(1), 36–54.
Barbu, C. (2019). Etude
des aspects du bilinguisme favorisant le développement des capacités de contrôle attentionnel et
exécutif. Unpublished Doctoral thesis, ULiège, Liège.
Barbu, C., Gillet, S., Orban, S., & Poncelet, M. (2018). The
impact of language switching frequency on attentional and executive functioning in proficient bilingual
adults. Psychologica
Belgica,
58
(1), 115–127.
Barbu, C., Gillet, S., Orban, S., & Poncelet, M. (2020). The
impact of language switching frequency on attentional and executive functioning in proficient German and French speaking
bilingual adults. Frontiers in
Psychology,
11
1, 1078.
Barbu, C., Gonzalez, A., Gillet, S., & Poncelet, M. (2019). Cognitive
advantage in children enrolled in a second-language immersion elementary school program for one
year. Psychologica
Belgica,
59
(1), 416–435.
Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A
developmental perspective on executive function. Child
Development,
81
(6), 1641–1660.
Bialystok, E. (2011). Reshaping
the mind: the benefits of bilingualism. Canadian Journal of Experimental
Psychology,
65
(4), 229–35.
Bialystok, E. (2015). Cognitive
complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child
Development,
70
(3), 636–644.
Bialystok, E., & Barac, R. (2012). Emerging
bilingualism: dissociating advantages for metalinguistic awareness and executive
control. Cognition,
122
(7), 67–73.
Björklund, S. (1997). Immersion
in Finland in the 1990s. A state of development and
expansion. In R. K. Jonhson, & M. Swain (Eds.), Immersion
education: International perspectives, pp.
85–101. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Boot, W. R., Kramer, A. F., Simons, D. J., Fabiani, M., & Gratton, G. (2008). The
effects of video game playing on attention, memory, and executive control. Acta
Psychologica,
129
(3), 387–398.
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual
experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental
Science,
11
(2), 282–298.
Castel, A. D., Pratt, J., & Drummond, E. (2005). The
effects of action video game experience on the time course of inhibition of return and the efficiency of visual
search. Acta
Psychologica,
119
(2), 217–230.
Choi, J. Y., Lee, J. S., & Oh, J. S. (2018). Examining
the oral language competency of children from Korean immigrant families in English-only and dual language immersion
schools. Journal of Early Childhood
Research,
16
(1), 32–51.
Coderre, E. L., & van Heuven, W. J. B. (2014). The
effect of script similarity on executive control in bilinguals. Frontiers in
Psychology, 5(
1070
).
da Rosa Piccolo, L., Arteche, A. X., Fonseca, R. P., Grassi-Oliveira, R., & Salles, J. F. (2016). Influence
of family socioeconomic on IQ, language, memory and executive functions of Brazilian
children. Psicologia: Reflexão e
Crítica,
29
(23).
Dajani, D. R., & Uddin, L. Q. (2015). Demystifying
cognitive flexibility: Implications for clinical and developmental neuroscience. Trends in
Neurosciences,
38
(9), 571–578.
de Vos, T. (1992). Tempo Test Rekenen. [Arithmetic Speed
Test]. Berkhout Nijmegen.
Diamond, A. (2012). Activities
and programs that improve children’s executive functions. Current Directions in Psychological
Science,
21
(5), 335–341.
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive
functions. Annual Review of
Psychology,
64
1, 135–168.
Diamond, A. & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions
Shown to Aid Executive Function Development in Children 4–12 Years
Old. Science,
333
(6045), 959–964.
Diamond, A., & Ling, D. S. (2016). Conclusions
about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite
much hype, do not. Development Cognitive
Neuroscience,
18
1, 34–48.
European Commission, Directorate-General for Education, Youth, Sport and
Culture. (2005). Content and Language Integrated Learning (CLIL) at school in
Europe. Publications Office.
Fernandez, M., Tartar, J. L., Padron, D., & Acosta, J. (2013). Neurophysiological
marker of inhibition distinguishes language groups on a non-linguistic executive function
test. Brain and
Cognition,
83
(3), 330–336.
Fleckenstein, J., Gebauer, S. K., & Möller, J. (2019). Promoting
mathematics achievement in one-way immersion: Performance development over four years of elementary
school. Contemporary Educational
Psychology,
56
1, 228–235.
Flores, R. D., Sanders, C. A., Duan, S. X., et al. (2022). Before/after
Bayes: A comparison of frequentist and Bayesian mixed-effects models in applied psychological
research. British Journal of
Psychology,
113
(4), 1164–1194.
Gillet, S., Barbu, C., & Poncelet, M. (2020). Exploration
of attentional and executive abilities in French-speaking children immersed in Dutch since 1, 2, 3, and 6
years, Frontiers in
Psychology,
11
1, 587574.
Gillet, S., Barbu, C.-A., & Poncelet, M. (2021). Early
bilingual immersion school program and cognitive development in French-speaking children: Effect of the second language
learned (English vs. Dutch) and exposition duration (2 vs. 5 years). PLoS
ONE,
16
(10).
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental
control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition,
1
(2), 67–81.
Green, D. W. & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language
control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive
Psychology,
25
(5), 515–530.
Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There
is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive
Psychology,
66
(2), 232–258.
Hackman, D. A., Gallop, R., Evans, G. W., & Farah, M. J. (2015). Socioeconomic
status and executive function: Developmental trajectories and mediation. Developmental
Science,
18
(5), 686–702.
Hartanto, A., Yang, H., and Yang, S. (2018). Bilingualism
positively predicts mathematical competence: evidence from two large-scale studies. Learning
and Individual
Differences,
61
1, 216–227.
Hiligsmann, P., Van Mensel, L., Galand, B., Mettewie, L., Meunier, F., Szmalec, A., Van Goethem, K., Bulon, A., De Smet, A., Hendrikx, I., & Simonis, M. (2017). Assessing
Content and Language Integrated Learning (CLIL) in French-speaking Belgium: linguistic, cognitive, and educational
perspectives. Les Cahiers De Recherche Du
Girsef, (109). 1–25. [URL]
Janus, M., Lee, Y., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2016). Effects
of short term music and second language training on executive control. Journal of Experimental
Child
Psychology,
144
1, 84–97.
Kalashnikova, M., & Mattock, K. (2014). Maturation
of executive functioning skills in early sequential bilingualism. International Journal of
Bilingual Education and
Bilingualism,
17
(1), 111–123.
Kaushanskaya, M., Gross, M., & Buac, M. (2014). Effects
of classroom bilingualism on task shifting, verbal memory, and word learning in
children. Developmental
Science,
17
(4), 564–583.
Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bogulski, C. A., and Valdes-Kroff, J. (2012). Juggling
two languages in one mind: what bilinguals tell us about language processing and its consequences for
cognition. In B. H. Ross (Ed), The
Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol.
56B (pp. 229–262). Academic Press.
Last, B. S., Lawson, G. M., Breiner, K., Steinberg, L., & Farah, M. J. (2018). Childhood
socioeconomic status and executive function in childhood and beyond. PLoS
ONE.
13
(
8
).
Lee, M. D., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2014). Bayesian
cognitive modeling: A practical course. Cambridge University Press.
Love, J., Selker, R., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Dropmann, D., Verhagen, A. J., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2015). JASP (Version
0.7).
Lyster, R. (2017). Content-Based
Language Teaching (1st ed.). New York: Routledge.
Miura, I. T., Okamoto, Y., Kim, C. C., Steere, M., & Fayol, M. (1993). First
graders’ cognitive representation of number and understanding of place value: Cross-national comparisons: France, Japan,
Korea, Sweden, and the United States. Journal of Educational
Psychology,
85
(1), 24–30.
Neveu, A., Crespo, K., Weismer, S. E., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2021). Does
long-term dual-language immersion affect children’s executive functioning? Journal of
Experimental Child
Psychology,
208
1, 105127.
Nicolay, A. C. (2012). Acquisition
d’une seconde langue dans un contexte d’immersion linguistique scolaire précoce : conditions et conséquences sur le plan du
développement cognitif. Unpublished Doctoral
thesis, ULiège, Liège.
Nicolay, A. C., & Poncelet, M. (2013). Cognitive
advantage in children enrolled in a second-language immersion elementary school program for 3
years. Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition,
16
(3), 597–607.
Nicolay, A. C., & Poncelet, M. (2015). Cognitive
benefits in children enrolled in an early bilingual immersion school: a follow up
study. Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition,
18
(4), 789–795.
Nishat, T. (2015). Influence
of bilingualism on simple arithmetic. Non-published thesis.
Ó Duibhir, P. (2009). The
spoken Irish of sixth-class pupils in Irish immersion schools. Dissertation
thesis, Trinity College, Dublin.
Oschwald, J., Schättin, A., Von Bastian, C. C., & Souza, A. S. (2018). Bidialectalism
and bilingualism: Exploring the role of language similarity as a link between linguistic ability and executive
control. Frontiers in
Psychology,
9
1.
Paap, K. R. (2018). Bilingualism
in cognitive science. In A. De Houwer & L. Ortega (Eds.), Handbook
of Bilingualism. Cambridge University Press.
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual
advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined
circumstances, Cortex,
69
1, 265–78.
Patkin, D. (2011). The
interplay of language and
mathematics, Pythagoras,
32
(2), 1–17.
Poarch, G. J., & van Hell, J. G. (2012). Executive
functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: evidence from second-language learners, bilinguals, and
trilinguals. Journal of Experimental
Psychology,
113
(4), 535–551.
Saalbach, H., Eckstein, D., Andri, N., Hobi, R., & Grabner, R. H. (2013). When
language of instruction and language of application differ: Cognitive costs of bilingual mathematics
learning. Learning and
Instruction,
26
1, 36–44.
Simonis, M. (2019). The
Cognitive Profile of Children Enrolled in Content and Language Integrated Learning in French-speaking
Belgium. Unpublished Doctoral
thesis, UCLouvain, Louvain-La-Neuve.
Simonis, M., Galand, B., Hiligsmann, P., Szmalec, A. (2023). Attentional
abilities of children enrolled in immersion education in French-speaking Belgium. International
Journal of Bilingual Education and
Bilingualism,
26
(
5
), 554–571.
Simonis, M., Van Der Linden, L., Galand, B., Hiligsmann, P., Szmalec, A. (2020). Executive
control performance and foreign-language proficiency associated with immersion education in French-speaking
Belgium. Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition,
23
(2), 355–370.
Turnbull, M., Hart, D., & Lapkin, S. (2000). French
immersion students’ performance on grade 3 and grade 6 provincial tests: Potential impacts on program
design. Toronto, Ontario: Final report submitted to Education Quality and Accountability Office (EQAO).
Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2007). A
practical solution to the pervasive problem of p values. Psychonomic Bulletin and
Review,
14
(
5
), 779–804.
Wagenmakers, E.-J., Verhagen, J., Ly, A., Bakker, M., Lee, M. D., Matzke, D., Rounder, J. N., & Morey, R. D. (2015). A
power fallacy. Behavioral Research
Methods,
47
(4), 913–917.
Walter, H. (2001). Honni
Soit Qui mal y Pense : L’incroyable histoire d’amour entre le français et
l’anglais. Laffont.
Ware, A. T., Kirkovski, M. & Lum, J. A. G. (2020). Meta-Analysis
Reveals a Bilingual Advantage That Is Dependent on Task and Age. Frontiers in
Psychology,
11
1, 1458.
Wilkinson, L. (2019). Learning
language and mathematics: A perspective from Linguistics and Education. Linguistics and
Education,
49
1, 86–95.
Woumans, E., Surmont, J., & Struys, S. (2016). The
longitudinal effect of bilingual immersion schooling on cognitive control and
intelligence. Language
Learning,
66
(
2
), 76–91.
Zimmermann, P., Gondan, M., & Fimm, B. (2002). KiTAP:
Test of attentional performance in children. Herzogenrath, Germany: Psytest.
Zuk, Z., Benjamin, C., Kenyon, A., & Gaab, N. (2014). Behavioral
and neural correlates of executive functioning in musicians and non-musicians. PLoS
ONE,
9
(6).