Article published In:
Journal of Immersion and Content-Based Language Education: Online-First ArticlesEFL lexical abilities in pre-primary education
Longitudinal effects of individual differences, quality and quantity of exposure
The increasing need for plurilingualism within the latest decades has led many schools to promote an early start
to Foreign Language (FL) teaching, in some cases as early as in pre-primary education. Research on pre-primary students to
understand FL development in instructed contexts is still scarce. This study examines (1) how English as a FL receptive vocabulary
develops over two years in pre-primary education learners, and (2) which internal and external factors influence it. Data were
collected from 71 Catalan-Spanish bilingual pre-primary students at three times: T1-September 2019
(M
age = 4;6), T2-September 2020 (M
age = 5;1), and T3-June 2021
(M
age = 5;9). EFL receptive vocabulary was measured with the PPVT-4 (Dunn & Dunn, 2007), and at T1 participants’ parents filled in a background questionnaire. Results
showed growth in participants’ lexical abilities from T1-T3. The strongest predictors of lexical development were gender, maternal
education level, school, and EFL out-of-school exposure.
Keywords: lexical abilities, English as a Foreign Language, CLIL, out-of-school exposure, individual differences, pre-primary education
Article outline
- 1.Introduction
- 1.1Individual differences (internal vs external factors)
- 1.2Quantity and quality of exposure
- 2.Methodology
- 2.1Study design
- 2.2Participants and intervention
- 2.3Data collection
- 2.4Data analysis
- 3.Results
- 3.1Overall lexical development
- 3.2Individual differences
- 3.3Quality and quantity of exposure
- 4.Discussion
- 4.1Overall lexical development
- 4.2Predictors of lexical development
- 5.Conclusions
- Acknowledgements
- Declaration of interest statement
- Data availability statement
- Note
-
References
Available under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives (CC BY-NC-ND) 4.0 license.
For any use beyond this license, please contact the publisher at [email protected].
Published online: 29 November 2024
https://doi.org/10.1075/jicb.24029.seg
https://doi.org/10.1075/jicb.24029.seg
References (75)
Agustín-Llach, M. P. (2015). Age
and type of instruction (CLIL vs. traditional EFL) in lexical development. International
Journal of English
Studies,
16
(1), 75–96.
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting
Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical
Software,
67
(1), 1–48.
Bruton, A. (2011). Is
CLIL so beneficial, or just selective? Re-evaluating some of the
research. System,
39
(4), 523–532.
Castellano-Risco, I., Alejo-González, R., & Piquer-Píriz, A. M. (2020). The
development of receptive vocabulary in CLIL vs EFL: Is the learning context the main
variable? System,
9
1.
Catalan, R. M. J. (2003). Sex
differences in L2 vocabulary learning strategies. International Journal of Applied
Linguistics,
13
(1), 54–77.
Cenoz, J., & Valencia, J. F. (1994). Additive
trilingualism: Evidence from the Basque country. Applied
Psycholinguistics,
15
(2), 195–207.
Dale, L., & Tanner, R. (2012). CLIL
activities. A resource for subject and language teaching. Cambridge University Press.
Dalton-Puffer, C. (2008). Outcomes
and processes in Content and Language Integrated Learning (CLIL): Current research from
Europe. In W. Delanoy, & L. Volkmann (Eds.), Future
perspectives for English language
teaching (pp. 139–157). Carl Winter.
Dalton-Puffer, C., Hüttner, J., & Llinares, A. (2022). CLIL
in the 21st Century. Retrospective and prospective challenges and opportunities. Journal of
Immersion and Content-Based Language
Education,
10
(2), 182–206.
David, A., & Wei, L. (2008). Individual
Differences in the Lexical Development of French–English Bilingual Children. International
Journal of Bilingual Education and
Bilingualism,
11
(5), 598–618.
De Wilde, V., Brysbaert, M., & Eyckmans, J. (2020). Learning
English through out-of-school exposure. Which levels of language proficiency are attained and which types of input are
important? Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition,
23
(1), 171185.
Delaney, J., & Devereux, P. J. (2021). Gender
and Educational Achievement: Stylized Facts and Causal Evidence. Center for Economic Policy
Research, Discussion paper No.
DP15753. Retrieved October 4,
2024, from [URL].
Dörnyei, Z. (2005). The
Psychology of the Language Learner: Individual Differences in Second Language
Acquisition. Lawrence Erlbaum.
Emilsson, K. H. (2021). The
relationship between extramural English exposure and receptive vocabulary knowledge of young Swedish L2 learners of
English. [Doctoral dissertation, Lund University] Lund University Publications. [URL]
European Commission. (1995). While paper
on education and training. Teaching and learning — Towards the learning society. Office of Official Publications of the European Communities.
. (2003). Promoting
language learning and linguistic diversity: An action plan 2004–2006. Office of Official Publications of the European Communities.
European
Commission. (2011). Commission staff working paper — Language learning at
pre-primary level: making it efficient and sustainable — A policy handbook.
Eurostat. (2024). Foreign language
learning statistics. Retrieved September 30, 2024, from [URL]
Eurydice European Unit. (2006). Content
and Language Integrated Learning (CLIL) at school in Europe. Directorate-General for Education and Culture of the European Commission.
. (2023). Key data
on teaching languages at school in Europe — 2023 edition. Publications Office of the European Union.
Farkas, G., & Beron, K. (2004). The
detailed age trajectory of oral vocabulary knowledge: Differences by class and race. Social
Science
Research,
33
(3), 464–497.
García Esteban, S. (2015). Soft
CLIL in infant education bilingual contexts in Spain. International Journal of Language and
Applied
Linguistics,
1
1, 30–36.
Gené-Gil, M., Juan-Garau, M., & Salazar-Noguera, J. (2016). A
methodology for longitudinal research on EFL written production: Capturing writing multidimensionality by combining
qualitative and quantitative procedures. Journal of Research Design and Linguistics and
Communication
Sciences,
3
(1), 23–48.
Generalitat de
Catalunya. (2023). Decret 21/2023, de 7 de febrer, d’ordenació dels
ensenyaments de l’educació infant [Decree 21/2023, February 7, by which the ordinance of the teaching in pre-primary education
in established]. Diari Oficial de la Generalitat de
Catalunya,
8851
1.
Goris, J., Denessen, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2019). Effects
of content and language integrated learning in Europe. A systematic review of longitudinal experimental
studies. European Educational Research
Journal,
18
(6), 675–695.
Hoff, E. (2006). How
social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental
review,
26
(1), 55–88.
Institut d’Estadística de
Catalunya (2018). Usos lingüístics de la població. Llengua inicial,
d’identificació i habitual. Retrieved July 8, 2024, from [URL]
(2019). Enquesta d’usos lingüístics de la
població. Retrieved July 8,
2024, from [URL]
Ikeda, M. (2013). Does
CLIL work for Japanese secondary school students? Potential for the ‘weak’ version of
CLIL. International CLIL Research
Journal,
2
(1), 31–43.
Ikeda, M., Izumi, S., Watanabe, Y., Pinner, R., & Davis, M. (2021). Soft
CLIL and English language teaching. Understanding Japanese policy, practice, and
implications. Routledge Handbooks.
Jafarigohar, M., Fivsar, H., & Etemad, P. (2022). The
effect of CLIL context on the primary education EFL receptive and productive lexical
growth. Language Teaching Research, Online
first.
Jiménez-Catalán, R. M., & Agustín-Llach, M. P. (2017). CLIL
or time? Lexical profiles of CLIL and non-CLIL EFL
learners. System,
66
1, 87–99.
Jiménez-Catalán, R. M., Ruiz de Zarobe, Y., & Cenoz, J. (2006). Vocabulary
profiles of English foreign language learners in English as a subject and as a vehicular
language. Views,
15
(3), 23–26.
Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical
period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second
language. Cognitive
psychology,
21
(1), 60–99.
Kassambara, A. (2023). rstatix:
Pipe-Friendly Framework for Basic Statistical Tests. R Package Version 0.7.2. [URL]
Lindgren, E. & Muñoz, C. (2013). The
influence of exposure, parents, and linguistic distance on young European learners’ foreign language
comprehension. International Journal of
Multilingualism,
10
(1), 105–129.
Llach, M. P. A., & Gallego, M. T. (2012). Vocabulary
knowledge development and gender differences in a second
language. Elia, 121, 45–75.
Lasagabaster, D., & Sierra, J. M. (2009). Language
attitudes in CLIL and international EFL classes. International CLIL Research
Journal,
1
(2), 4–17.
Lenth, R., Singmann, H., Love, J., Buerkner, P., & Herve, M. (2023). Estimated
Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means. R package version 1.9.0. [URL]
Leona, N. L., van Koert, M. J. H., van der Molen, M. W., Rispens, J. E., Tijms, J., & Snellings, P. (2021). Explaining
individual differences in young English language learners’ vocabulary knowledge: The role of Extramural English Exposure and
motivation. System,
96
1.
Llinares, A., & Morton, T. (2010). Historical
explanations as situated practice in content and language integrated learning. Classroom
Discourse,
1
(1), 46–65.
Lorenzo, F., Casal, S., & Moore, P. (2010). The
effects of content and language integrated learning in European education: Key findings from the Andalusian bilingual sections
evaluation project. Applied
Linguistics,
31
(3), 418–442.
Lüdecke, D., Ben-Shachar, M., Patil, I., Waggoner, P., & Makowski, D. (2021). Performance:
An R Package for Assessment, Comparison and Testing of Statistical Models. Journal of Open
Source
Software,
6
(60), 3139.
Muñoz, C. (2008). Symmetries
and asymmetries of age effects in naturalistic and instructed L2 learning. Applied
Linguistics,
29
(4), 578–596.
(2011). Input
and long-term effects of starting age in foreign language learning. International Review of
Applied Linguistics in Language
Teaching,
49
(2), 113–133.
Navés, T. (2009). Effective
Content and Language Integrated (CLIL) Programmes. In Y. Ruiz de Zarobe and R. M. Jiménez Catalán, (Eds.), Content
and Language Integrated Learning. Evidence from Research in
Europe (pp. 22–40). Multilingual Matters.
Navés, T., & Muñoz, C. (1999). CLIL
experiences in Spain. In D. Marsh, and G. Langé, (Eds.), Implementing
Content and Language Integrated Learning. TIE-CLIL.
Nieuwenhuis, R., Te Grotenhuis, M., & Pelzer, B. (2012). Influence.ME:
Tools for Detecting Influential Data in Mixed Effects Models. R
Journal,
4
(2), 38–47. [URL].
Paradis, J. (2019). English
second language acquisition from early childhood to adulthood: The role of age, first language, cognitive, and input
factors. In Proceedings of the
BUCLD (Vol. 431, pp. 11–26).
(2011). Individual
differences in child English second language acquisition: Comparing child-internal and child-external
factors. Linguistic approaches to
bilingualism,
1
(3), 213–237.
Pérez Cañado, M. L. (2018). CLIL
and educational levels: A longitudinal study on the impact of CLIL on language outcomes. Porta
Linguarium,
29
1, 51–70.
R Core Team (2023). R: A Language and
Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical computing, Vienna, Austria. [URL]
Revelle, W. (2024). psych:
Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research. R package version
2.4.3, [URL]
Rua, P. L. (2006). The
sex variable in foreign language learning: an integrative approach. Porta
Linguarum
6
(1), 99–144.
Sagasta, M. P. (2003). Acquiring
writing skills in a third language: The positive effects of bilingualism. International Journal
of
Bilingualism,
7
(1), 27–42.
San Isidro, X., & Lasagabaster, D. (2019). The
impact of CLIL on pluriliteracy development and content learning in a rural multilingual setting: A longitudinal
study. Language Teaching
Research,
23
(5), 584–602.
Sanz, C. (2000). Bilingual
education enhances third language acquisition: Evidence from Catalonia. Applied
Psycholinguistics,
21
(1), 23–44.
(2008). Predicting enhanced L3 learning in bilingual contexts: The role of biliteracy. In C. Pérez-Vidal, M. Juan-Garau, & A. Bel (Eds.), A portrait of the young in the new multilingual Spain (pp. 220–240). Multilingual Matters.
Segura, M., Barón, J., & Roquet, H. (2022). Productive
vocabulary development in pre-primary through soft CLIL. Open
Linguistics,
8
(1), 297–327.
Soto-Corominas, A., Roquet, H., & Segura, M. (2024). The
effects of CLIL and sources of individual differences on receptive and productive EFL skills at the onset of primary
school. Applied
Linguistics,
45
(2), 364–382.
Soto-Corominas, A., Segura, M., Roquet, H., Navarro, N., & Met, Y. E. (2024). Third
language English performance at the onset of schooling: Effects of bilingualism and exposure in Catalan-Spanish bilingual
children. International Journal of Multilingualism (online
first).
Sun, H., Steinkrauss, R., Tendeiro, J., & De Bot, K. (2016). Individual
differences in very young children’s English acquisition in China: Internal and external
factors. Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition, 19(3), 550–566.
Sun, H., Steinkrauss, R., Wieling, M., & De Bot, K. (2018). Individual
differences in very young Chinese children’s English vocabulary breadth and semantic depth: Internal and external
factors. International Journal of Bilingual Education and
Bilingualism,
21
(4), 405–425.
Sundqvist, P. (2024). Extramural
English as an individual difference variable in L2 research: Methodology matters. Annual Review
of Applied Linguistics, online first.
Sylvén, L. K. (2010). Teaching
in English or English teaching? On the effects of content and language integrated learning on Swedish learners’ incidental
vocabulary acquisition [Doctoral dissertation]. University of Gothenburg.
Tedick, D. J., & Lyster, R. (2020). Scaffolding
language development in immersion and dual language
classrooms. Routledge.
Thordardottir, E. (2011). The
relationship between bilingual exposure and vocabulary development. International Journal of
Bilingualism,
15
(4), 426–445.
Unsworth, S., Persson, L., Prins, T., & De Bot, K. (2015). An
investigation of factors affecting early foreign language learning in the Netherlands. Applied
linguistics,
36
(5), 527–548.
Van Mensel, L., & Galand, B. (2022). Testing
the predictive power of executive functions, motivation, and input on second language vocabulary acquisition: a prospective
study. European Journal of Psychology of
Education,
38
(2), 475–494.
Venables, W. N., & Ripley, B. D. (2002). Modern
Applied Statistics with S (4th
ed.) Springer. [URL].
Vraciu, A., & Marsol, A. (2023). Content-specific
vocabulary in CLIL: Exploring L2 learning outcomes in a primary education programme in
Catalonia. Language Teaching Research, Online
first.
Wickham, H., Davis, V., Girlich, M., & Usher, K. (2024). tidyr:
Tidy Messy Data. R package version 1.3.1. [URL]