Article published In:
Journal of Second Language Pronunciation
Vol. 1:2 (2015) ► pp.181210
References (60)
Agresti, A. (2002). Categorical data analysis (2nd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons, Inc. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Altmann, H. (2006). The perception and production of second language stress: A cross-linguistic experimental study. Doctoral dissertation, University of Delaware.
Altmann, H., & Vogel, I. (2002). L2 acquisition of stress: The role of L1. Paper presented at the DGfS Annual Meeting “Multilingualism Today” . Mannheim, Germany.
Audacity Team. (2012). Audacity. Version 2.0.0. Audio editor and recorder. Available from: [URL] (Accessed 26/04/2012).
Baker, M. (2008). The macroparameter in a microparametric world. In T. Biberauer (Ed.), The limits of syntactic variation, 132 (pp. 351–374). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bradlow, A.R., Pisoni, D.B., Yamada, R.A., & Tohkura, Y. (1997). Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/: IV. Some effects of perceptual learning on speech production. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1011, 2299–2310. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bradlow, A.R., Yamada, R.A., Pisoni, D.B., & Tohkura, Y. (1999). Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/: Long-term retention of learning in perception and production. Percept Psychophys, 61(5), 977–985. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bullock, B., & Lord, G. (2003). Analogy as a learning tool in second language acquisition: The case of Spanish stress. In A.T. Pérez- Leroux & Y. Roberge (Eds.), Romance linguistics: Theory and Acquisition (pp. 281–297). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. (1973). The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 121, 335–359. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clopper, C.G. (2002). Frequency of stress patterns in English: A computational analysis. Indiana University Linguistics Club Working Papers 2 .
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cooper, N., Cutler, A. & Wales, R. (2002). Constraints of lexical stress on lexical access in English: Evidence from native and nonnative listeners. Language and Speech, 451, 207–228. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cortés, A., & Menegotto, A.C. (2000). Los contenidos culturales en los cursos de español como lengua segunda y como lengua extranjera: la experiencia del Programa Mar del Plata [Cultural content in courses of Spanish as a Second and Foreign language: the experience of the Mar del Plata Program]. In M. Franco, C. Soler, J. de Cos, M. Rivas, & F. Ruiz (Eds.), Proceedings of the X Congreso de ASELE. Nuevas perspectivas en la enseñanza del español como lengua extranjera (pp. 193–202). Cádiz, Spain: Rivas y Ruiz, Servicio de Publicaciones de la Universidad de Cádiz.Google Scholar
Cutler, A. (1986). Forbear is a homophone: Lexical prosody does not constrain lexical access. Language & Speech, 291, 201–220. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2009). Greater sensitivity to prosodic goodness in nonnative than in native listeners (L). Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1251, 3522–3525. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., Norris, D., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2004). Phonemic repertoire and similarity within the vocabulary. In S.H. Kin & M. Jin Bae (Eds.), Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Spoken Language Processing, 11, 65–68. Seoul, South Korea: Sunjin Printing Co.Google Scholar
Cutler, A., & Pasveer, D. (2006). Explaining cross-linguistic differences in effects of lexical stress on spoken-word recognition. In R. Hoffman & H. Mixdorff (Eds.), Speech Prosody, Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference (pp. 237–400). Dresden, Germany: TUD Press.
Cutler, A., & Van Donselaar, W. (2001). Voornaam is not a homophone: Lexical prosody and lexical access in Dutch. Language and Speech, 441, 171–195. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Delattre, P. (1966). A comparison of syllable length conditioning among languages. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 4(14), 183–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diccionario integral del español de la Argentina. (2008). [Comprehensive dictionary of Argentinian Spanish]. Buenos Aires, Argentina: Tinta fresca.Google Scholar
Dupoux, E., Pallier, C., Sebastian, N., & Mehler, J. (1997). A destressing ‘deafness’ in French?. Journal of Memory and Language, 361, 406–421. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dupoux, E., & Peperkamp, S. (2002). Fossil markers of language development: phonological ‘deafnesses’ in adult speech processing. In J. Durand & B. Laks (Eds.), Phonetics, phonology, and cognition, 168–190. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dupoux, E., Peperkamp, S., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2001). A robust method to study stress deafness. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 110(3), 1606–1618. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dupoux, E., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Navarrete, E., & Peperkamp, S. (2007). Persistent stress deafness: The case of French learners of Spanish. Cognition, 1061, 682–706. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Field, J. (2005). Intelligibility and the listener: The role of lexical stress. TESOL Quarterly, 39(3), 399–423. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, J.N. (1990). Spanish. In B. Comrie (Ed.), The World´s major languages (pp. 236–259). Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Harris, A. (1983). Syllable structure and stress in Spanish: A nonlinear analysis. Cambridge: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hualde, J.I. (2005). The sounds of Spanish. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Huck, S.W. (2009). Reading statistics and research (5th ed.). New York, NY: Pearson Education.Google Scholar
Jaeger, T.F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and toward logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 591, 434–446. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kijak, A. (2009). How stressful is L2 stress? A cross-linguistic study of L2 stress perception and production of metrical systems. Doctoral dissertation, Utrecht University.
Lively, S.E., Logan, J.D., & Pisoni, D.B. (1993). Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/: II. The role of phonetic environment and talker variability in learning new perceptual categories. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 941, 1242–1255. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Logan, J.D., Lively, S.E., & Pisoni, D.B. (1991). Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/: a first report. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 89(2), 874–886. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lord, G. (2010). The combined effects of immersion and instruction on second language pronunciation. Foreign Language Annals, 43(3), 488–503. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Llisterri, J. Machuca, M., de la Mota, C., Riera, M., & Ríos, A. (2003) The perception of lexical stress in Spanish. In M.J. Solé, D. Recasens, & J. Romero (Eds.), Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 2023–2026). Barcelona, Spain: Causal Productions.Google Scholar
Menegotto, A. (2005a). Morfología verbal del español del Río de la Plata. [Verbal morphology of River Plate Spanish]. Mar del Plata, Argentina: Finisterre editores.Google Scholar
. (2005b). Trabajo por proyectos en la enseñanza de español a extranjeros: el problema de la evaluación [Project work in the teaching of Spanish to foreign students: The assessment problem]. 1o coloquio CELU . Buenos Aires, Argentina.
Menegotto, A.C. (1999). Del aula a la calle: la integración de objetivos y la transferencia de habilidades en el Programa Mar del Plata [From the classroom to the street: integration of objectives and transfer of skills in the Programa Mar del Plata]. In J. Juliá, M.C. Aldrey, & J.F.M. Caneda (Eds.), IX Congreso Internacional de A.S.E.L.E. (pp. 863–864). Santiago de Compostela, Spain: Xunta de Galicia - Editorial Difusión.Google Scholar
. (2007). Successful project work. In K.N. Ware (Ed.), Recipes for success in foreign language teaching. LINCOM Europa.Google Scholar
Menegotto, A.C., & Cortés, A. (2014). Documentos fundacionales. Programa Mar del Plata de Español para Extranjeros. [Founding papers. SFL in the Programa Mar del Plata]. Mar del Plata, Argentina: Universidad Nacional de Mar del Plata.Google Scholar
Nishi, K., & Kewley-Port, D. (2007). Training Japanese listeners to perceive American English vowels: influence of training sets. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 501, 1496–1509. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2008). Non-native speech perception training using vowel subsets: Effects of vowels in sets and order of training. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 50(6), 1480–1493. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ortega-Llebaria, M. (2007). Perception of stress in Spanish by L1 and L2 speakers. Paper presented at the 6th International Symposium on Bilingualism , University of Hamburg.
Ortega-Llebaria, M., Gu, H., & Fan, J. (2013). English speakers’ perception of Spanish lexical stress: Context-driven L2 perception. Journal of Phonetics, 411, 186–197. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peperkamp, S., & Dupoux, E. (2002). A typological study of stress deafness. In C. Gussenhoven & N. Warner (Eds.), Laboratory Phonology, 7 (pp. 203–240). Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peperkamp, S., Vendelin, I., & Dupoux, E. (2010). Perception of predictable stress: A cross-linguistic investigation. Journal of Phonetics, 381, 422–430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Post da Silveira, A. (2011). Frequency as (dis) advantage to word stress acquisition. ICPhS 2011. 17th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 1634–1637). Hong Kong, China.
Quilis, A. (1984). Métrica española. [Spanish Metrics]. Barcelona, Spain: Ariel.Google Scholar
Raaijmakers, J., Schrijnemakers, J., & Gremmen, F. (1999). How to deal with “The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy”: Common misconceptions and alternative solutions. Journal of Memory and Language, 411, 416–426. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Real Academia Española. (2011). Nueva gramática de la lengua española: fonética y fonología. [New grammar of Spanish: Phonetics and phonology]. Madrid, Spain: Espasa-Calpe.Google Scholar
Romanelli, S. (2015). Percepción y producción de las vocales y el acento del español como L2 por hablantes nativos de inglés. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Universidad de Buenos Aires.
Saalfeld, A. (2012). Teaching L2 Spanish stress. Foreign Language Annals, 45(2), 283–303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwab, S., & Llisterri, J. (2014). Does training make French speakers more able to identify lexical stress? In COPAL . Concordia Working Papers in Applied Linguistics, 51, 624–636.Google Scholar
Soto-Faraco, S., Sebastián-Gallés, N., & Cutler, A. (2001). Segmental and suprasegmental mismatch in lexical access. Journal of Memory and Language, 451, 412–432. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tyler, M., & Cutler, A. (2009). Cross-language differences in cue use for speech segmentation. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 126(1), 367–376. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Donselaar, W., Koster, M., & Cutler, A. (2005). Exploring the role of lexical stress in lexical recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A1, 251–273. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vogel, I. (2000). The acquisition of prosodic phonology: Challenges for the L2 learner. Paper presented at “Structure, Acquisition, and Change of Grammars: Phonological and Syntactic Aspects” . Hamburg, Germany.
Wang, Y., Jongman, A., & Sereno, J. (2003). Acoustic and perceptual evaluation of Mandarin tone productions before and after perceptual training. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 113(2), 1033–1043. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wang, Y., Spence, M., Jongman, A., & Sereno, J. (1999). Training American listeners to perceive Mandarin tones. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106(6), 3649–3658. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zagona, K.T. (1988). Verb phrase syntax: A parametric study of English and Spanish. Dordrecht, Boston: Kluwer Academic Publisher. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (11)

Cited by 11 other publications

Nagle, Charlie & Germán Zárate-Sández
2024. The Phonetics and Phonology of Adult L2 Learners After Study Abroad. In The Cambridge Handbook of Bilingual Phonetics and Phonology,  pp. 542 ff. DOI logo
Romanelli, Sofía & Camilo Vélez-Agudelo
2024. Variation in formant dynamics of L1 and L2 Spanish vowels. Lingua 301  pp. 103685 ff. DOI logo
Morales-Front, Alfonso
Sierra, Elkin, Gabriela Martinez Loyola, Takuya Kimura, Ryan Stevenson, Rajiv Rao & Yasaman Rafat
2023. The effects of context of learning on the perception of Spanish stress by Japanese speakers. Journal of Speech Sciences 12  pp. e023004 ff. DOI logo
Ortín, Ramsés & Miquel Simonet
2022. PHONOLOGICAL PROCESSING OF STRESS BY NATIVE ENGLISH SPEAKERS LEARNING SPANISH AS A SECOND LANGUAGE. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 44:2  pp. 460 ff. DOI logo
Ortín, Ramsés & Miquel Simonet
2023. Perceptual Sensitivity to Stress in Native English Speakers Learning Spanish as a Second Language. Laboratory Phonology 14:1 DOI logo
Levis, John M.
2019. Teaching-oriented research. Journal of Second Language Pronunciation 5:1  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
McAndrews, Mark
2019. Short periods of instruction improve learners’ phonological categories for L2 suprasegmental features. System 82  pp. 151 ff. DOI logo
Isabelli-García, Christina, Jennifer Bown, John L. Plews & Dan P. Dewey
2018. Language learning and study abroad. Language Teaching 51:4  pp. 439 ff. DOI logo
Romanelli, Sofía, Andrea Menegotto & Ron Smyth
2018. Stress-Induced Acoustic Variation in L2 and L1 Spanish Vowels. Phonetica 75:3  pp. 190 ff. DOI logo
Sadat‐Tehrani, Nima
2017. Teaching English Stress: A Case Study. TESOL Journal 8:4  pp. 943 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 22 november 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.