Part of
Structures, Strategies and Beyond: Studies in honour of Adriana Belletti
Edited by Elisa Di Domenico, Cornelia Hamann and Simona Matteini
[Linguistik Aktuell/Linguistics Today 223] 2015
► pp. 303320
References (30)
References
Adams, Catherine. 1990. Syntactic comprehension in children with expressive language impairment. British Journal of Disorders of Communication 25: 149–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Belletti, Adriana. 2014. Notes on passive object relatives. In Functional Structure from Top to Toe: The Cartography of Syntactic Structures, Vol. 9, Peter Svenonius (ed.), 97–114. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Belletti, Adriana & Contemori, Carla. 2010. Intervention and attraction. On the production of subject and object relatives by Italian young children and adults. In Language Acquisition and Development: Proceedings of Gala 2009, João Costa, Ana Castro, Maria Lobo & Fernanda Pratas (eds), 39−52. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Belletti, Adriana, Friedmann, Naama, Brunato, Dominique & Rizzi, Luigi. 2012. Does gender make a difference? Comparing the effect of gender on children’s comprehension of relative clauses in Hebrew and Italian. Lingua 122(10): 1053–1069. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Belletti, Adriana & Rizzi, Luigi. 2012. Ways of avoiding intervention: Some thoughts on object relatives, passive and control. In Rich Languages from Poor Inputs, Massimo Piattelli-Palmarini & Robert C. Berwick (eds), 115–126. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cipriani, Paola, Bottari, Piero, Chilosi, Anna Maria & Pfanner, Lucia. 1998. A longitudinal perspective on the study of specific language impairment: The long term follow-up of an Italian child. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 33: 245–280. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Collins, Chris. 2005. A smuggling approach to the passive in English. Syntax 8(2): 81–120. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Contemori, Carla & Belletti, Adriana. 2014. Relatives and passive object relatives in Italian-speaking children and adults: Intervention in production and comprehension. Applied Psycholinguistics 35(6): 1021–1053. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crawford, John R. & Howell, David C. 1998. Regression equations in clinical neuropsychology: An evaluation of statistical methods for comparing predicted and observed scores. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 20: 755–762. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Delage, Helene, Monjauze, Cecile, Hamann, Cornelia & Tuller, Laurie. 2008. Relative clauses in atypical acquisition of French. In Language Acquisition and Development: Proceedings of GALA 2007, Anna Gavarró & M. João Freitas (eds), 166–176. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Delage, Helene & Tuller, Laurie. 2007. Language development and mild-to-moderate hearing loss: Does language normalize with age? Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 50: 1–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fattal, Iris, Friedmann, Naama & Fattal-Valevski, Aviva. 2011. The crucial role of thiamine in the development of syntax and lexical retrieval: a study of infantile thiamine deficiency. Brain 134: 1720–1739. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friedmann, Naama. 2000. PETEL: a sentence repetition test. Tel Aviv University.Google Scholar
Friedmann, Naama, Belletti, Adriana, Tuller, Laurie, Costa, João, Guasti, Maria Teresa, Haman, Ewa & Varlokosta, Spyridoula. 2012. The acquisition of relative clause production in sixteen languages. Ms, COST A33.
Friedmann, Naama, Belletti, Adriana & Rizzi, Luigi. 2009. Relativized relatives: Types of intervention in the acquisition of A-bar dependencies. Lingua 119: 67–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friedmann, Naama & Novogrodsky, Rama. 2004. The acquisition of relative clause comprehension in Hebrew: A study of SLI and normal development. Journal of Child Language 31: 661–681. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2007. Is the movement deficit in syntactic SLI related to traces or to thematic role transfer? Brain and Language 101: 50–63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2011. Which questions are most difficult to understand? The comprehension of Wh questions in three subtypes of SLI. Lingua 121: 367–382. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friedmann, Naama & Szterman, Ronit. 2006. Syntactic movement in orally-trained children with hearing impairment. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 11(1): 56–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2011. The comprehension and production of wh questions in children with hearing impairment. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 16(2): 212–235. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grillo, Nino. 2005. Minimality effects in agrammatic comprehension. In Proceedings of ConSOLE XIII, Sylvia Blaho, Luis Vicente & Erik Schoorlemmer (eds), 107–120. Leiden: University of Leiden.Google Scholar
Håkansson, Gisela & Hansson, Kristina. 2000. Comprehension and production of relative clauses: A comparison between Swedish impaired and unimpaired children. Journal of Child Language 27: 313–333. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levy, Hagar & Friedmann, Naama. 2009. Treatment of syntactic movement in syntactic SLI: A case study. First Language 29: 15–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marinellie, Sally A. 2004. Complex syntax used by school-age children with specific language impairment (SLI) in child-adult conversation. Journal of Communication Disorders 37: 517–533. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Novogrodsky, Rama & Friedmann, Naama. 2006. The production of relative clauses in syntactic SLI: A window to the nature of the impairment. Advances in Speech-Language Pathology 8(4): 364–375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stavrakaki, Stavroula. 2001. Comprehension of reversible relative clauses in specifically language impaired and normally developing Greek children. Brain and Language 77: 419–431. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Szterman, Ronit & Friedmann, Naama. 2014. Relative clause reading in hearing impairment: Different profiles of syntactic impairment. Frontiers in Psychology: Language Sciences, 5(1229): 1–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2015. Insights into the syntactic deficit of children with hearing impairment from a sentence repetition task. In Language Acquisition and Development: Proceedings of GALA 2013, Cornelia Hamann & Esther Ruigendijk (eds), 492–505. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Press.Google Scholar
van der Lely, Heather K.J. 1997. Language and cognitive development in a grammatical SLI boy: Modularity and innateness. Journal of Neurolinguistics 10: 75–107. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Yachini, Maya, Szterman, Ronit & Friedmann, Naama. 2013, July. The effect of intervention on comprehension and production in developmental syntactic impairment. Presented in the 12th IMAM conference, Language and Brain Lab, Tel Aviv University.
Cited by (12)

Cited by 12 other publications

Belletti, Adriana
2024. Truncation versus reduction in development. In Rich Descriptions and Simple Explanations in Morphosyntax and Language Acquisition,  pp. 129 ff. DOI logo
Huang, Rui, Jeannette Schaeffer & Xiaowei He
2024.  Mandarin non-interrogative wh -words distinguish between children with Developmental Language Disorder and Language-Impaired autistic children . Language Acquisition  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Lai, Jane, Angel Chan & Evan Kidd
2024. Production of relative clauses in Cantonese-speaking children with and without Developmental Language Disorder. Brain and Language 254  pp. 105425 ff. DOI logo
Lai, Jane, Angel Chan, Evan Kidd & Iftikhar Ahmed Khan
2023. Relative clause comprehension in Cantonese-speaking children with and without developmental language disorder. PLOS ONE 18:11  pp. e0288021 ff. DOI logo
Sukenik, Nufar, Eléonore Morin, Naama Friedmann, Philippe Prevost & Laurice Tuller
2021. Coconuts and curtain cakes: The production of wh-questions in ASD. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments 6 DOI logo
Abed Ibrahim, Lina & István Fekete
2019. What Machine Learning Can Tell Us About the Role of Language Dominance in the Diagnostic Accuracy of German LITMUS Non-word and Sentence Repetition Tasks. Frontiers in Psychology 9 DOI logo
Belletti, Adriana & Claudia Manetti
2019. Topics and passives in Italian-speaking children and adults. Language Acquisition 26:2  pp. 153 ff. DOI logo
Sukenik, Nufar & Naama Friedmann
2018. ASD Is Not DLI: Individuals With Autism and Individuals With Syntactic DLI Show Similar Performance Level in Syntactic Tasks, but Different Error Patterns. Frontiers in Psychology 9 DOI logo
Hamann, Cornelia & Lina Abed Ibrahim
2017. Methods for Identifying Specific Language Impairment in Bilingual Populations in Germany. Frontiers in Communication 2 DOI logo
Ruigendijk, Esther & Naama Friedmann
2017. A Deficit in Movement-Derived Sentences in German-Speaking Hearing-Impaired Children. Frontiers in Psychology 8 DOI logo
Thornton, Rosalind, Kelly Rombough, Jasmine Martin & Linda Orton
2016. Negative sentences in children with specific language impairment. First Language 36:3  pp. 228 ff. DOI logo
Szterman, Ronit & Naama Friedmann
2014. Relative clause reading in hearing impairment: different profiles of syntactic impairment. Frontiers in Psychology 5 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 21 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.