References
Bock, J. K
1986Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 355–387. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
1989Closed-class immanence in sentence production. Cognition, 31, 163–186. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K., & Loebell, H
1990Framing sentences. Cognition, 35, 1–39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., Pickering M. J., & Cleland, A. A
2000Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue. Cognition 75, B13–B25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cai, Z. G., Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P
2012Mapping concepts to syntax: Evidence from structural priming in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 833–849. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cai, Z.G., Pickering, M.J., Yan, H., & Branigan, H.P
2011Lexical and syntactic representations in closely related languages: Evidence from Cantonese-Mandarin bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 65(4), 431–445. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cleland, A. A., & Pickering, M. J
2003The use of lexical and syntactic information in language production: Evidence from the priming of noun-phrase structure. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 214–230. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Corley, M., & Scheepers, C
2002Syntactic priming in English sentence production: Categorical and latency evidence from an Internet-based study. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 126–131. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, V. S
2003The persistence of optional complementizer production: Why saying ‘‘that’’ is not saying ‘‘that’’ at all. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 379–398. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fox Tree, J. E., & Meijer, P. J. A
1999Building syntactic structure in speaking. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 71–92. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A
1995Construction: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
2006Construction at Work: The Nature of Generalization in Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A., & Jackendoff, R
2004The English resultative as a family of constructions. Language 80, 532–568. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gu, Y.
1999双宾语结构 [Double object constructions]. In Universals and specifics: Controversy in Chinese linguistics (共性与个性:汉语语言学中的争议), L. Xu (ed.). Beijing: Beijing Language Culture University Press.Google Scholar
Hartsuiker, R.J., & Kolk H. H. J.
1998Syntactic persistence in Dutch. Language and Speech 41, 143–184. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Her, O.
2006Justifying part-of-speech assignments for Mandarin gei. Lingua 116 (8), 1274–1302. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hwang, H., & Chun, E.
2018Influence of social perception and social monitoring on structural priming. Cognitive Science, 42(51), 303–313. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hwang, H., & Shin, J-A.
2019Cumulative effects of syntactic experience in a between- and a within-language context: Evidence for implicit learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 109, 104054. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huang, C., & Ahrens K.
1999The function and category of gei in Mandarin ditransitive constructions. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 27(2), 1–26.Google Scholar
Kaan, E., & Chun, E.
2017Priming and adaptation in native speakers and second-language learners. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(2), 228–242. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaschak, M. P., Loney, R. A., & Borreggine, K. L.
2006Recent experience affects the strength of structural priming. Cognition, 99(3), B73–B82. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Konopka A.E., & Bock K.
2009Lexical or syntactic control of sentence formulation? Structural generalizations from idiom production. Cognitive Psychology, 58(1), 68–101. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Li, A.
1990Order and Constituency in Mandarin Chinese. Kluwer, Dordrecht. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Li, C., & Thompson S.
1981Mandarin Chinese: A Functional Reference Grammar. University of California Press, Berkeley and Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Lin, T.-H., & Huang Y.-S.
2015Structures of the Mandarin gei constructions. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 24(3), 309–388. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liu, D.
2001Hanyu geiyulei shuangjiwu jiegou de leixingxue kaocha [A typological study on the double-object construction of Chinese give verbs]. Zhongguo Yuwen 5.Google Scholar
Liu, F.
2006Dative constructions in Chinese. Language and Linguistics, 7(4). 863–904.Google Scholar
Pickering, M.J., & Branigan, H.P.
1998The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 633–651. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Ferreira, V. S.
2008Structural priming: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 427–459. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Scheepers C.
2003Syntactic priming of relative clause attachments: persistence of structural configuration in sentence production. Cognition 89, 179–205. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Shen, Y., He Y., & Gu. Y.
2001生成语法理论与汉语语法研究 [Generative Grammar and Chinese Grammar Research]. Harbin: Harbin Educational Press.Google Scholar
Tang, T.-C.
1978Double object constructions in Chinese. In Proceedings of Symposium on Chinese Linguistics, Linguistics Institute of the Linguistics Society of America 1977, Robert L. Cheng, Li Ying-Che & Ting-Chi Tang (eds.), 67–96. Taipei: Student.Google Scholar
Ting, J., & Chang M.
2004The category of gei in Mandarin Chinese and grammaticalization. Taiwan Journal of Linguistics, 2 (2), 45–74.Google Scholar
Zhang, N.
1998Argument interpretation in the ditransitive construction. Nordic Journal of Linguistics 21, 179–209. CrossrefGoogle Scholar