References (34)
References
Bock, J. K. 1986. Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 355–387. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 1989. Closed-class immanence in sentence production. Cognition, 31, 163–186. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K., & Loebell, H. 1990. Framing sentences. Cognition, 35, 1–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., Pickering M. J., & Cleland, A. A. 2000. Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue. Cognition 75, B13–B25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cai, Z. G., Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P. 2012. Mapping concepts to syntax: Evidence from structural priming in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 833–849. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cai, Z.G., Pickering, M.J., Yan, H., & Branigan, H.P. 2011. Lexical and syntactic representations in closely related languages: Evidence from Cantonese-Mandarin bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 65(4), 431–445. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cleland, A. A., & Pickering, M. J. 2003. The use of lexical and syntactic information in language production: Evidence from the priming of noun-phrase structure. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 214–230. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Corley, M., & Scheepers, C. 2002. Syntactic priming in English sentence production: Categorical and latency evidence from an Internet-based study. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 126–131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, V. S. 2003. The persistence of optional complementizer production: Why saying ‘‘that’’ is not saying ‘‘that’’ at all. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 379–398. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fox Tree, J. E., & Meijer, P. J. A. 1999. Building syntactic structure in speaking. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 71–92. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. 1995. Construction: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
. 2006. Construction at Work: The Nature of Generalization in Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A., & Jackendoff, R. 2004. The English resultative as a family of constructions. Language 80, 532–568. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gu, Y. 1999. 双宾语结构 [Double object constructions]. In Universals and specifics: Controversy in Chinese linguistics (共性与个性:汉语语言学中的争议), L. Xu (ed.). Beijing: Beijing Language Culture University Press.Google Scholar
Hartsuiker, R.J., & Kolk H. H. J. 1998. Syntactic persistence in Dutch. Language and Speech 41, 143–184. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Her, O. 2006. Justifying part-of-speech assignments for Mandarin gei. Lingua 116 (8), 1274–1302. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hwang, H., & Chun, E. 2018. Influence of social perception and social monitoring on structural priming. Cognitive Science, 42(51), 303–313. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hwang, H., & Shin, J-A. 2019. Cumulative effects of syntactic experience in a between- and a within-language context: Evidence for implicit learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 109, 104054. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huang, C., & Ahrens K. 1999. The function and category of gei in Mandarin ditransitive constructions. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 27(2), 1–26.Google Scholar
Kaan, E., & Chun, E. 2017. Priming and adaptation in native speakers and second-language learners. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(2), 228–242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaschak, M. P., Loney, R. A., & Borreggine, K. L. 2006. Recent experience affects the strength of structural priming. Cognition, 99(3), B73–B82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Konopka A.E., & Bock K. 2009. Lexical or syntactic control of sentence formulation? Structural generalizations from idiom production. Cognitive Psychology, 58(1), 68–101. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, A., 1990. Order and Constituency in Mandarin Chinese. Kluwer, Dordrecht. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, C., & Thompson S. 1981. Mandarin Chinese: A Functional Reference Grammar. University of California Press, Berkeley and Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Lin, T.-H., & Huang Y.-S. 2015. Structures of the Mandarin gei constructions. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 24(3), 309–388. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liu, D. 2001. Hanyu geiyulei shuangjiwu jiegou de leixingxue kaocha [A typological study on the double-object construction of Chinese give verbs]. Zhongguo Yuwen 5.Google Scholar
Liu, F. 2006. Dative constructions in Chinese. Language and Linguistics, 7(4). 863–904.Google Scholar
Pickering, M.J., & Branigan, H.P. 1998. The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 633–651. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Ferreira, V. S. 2008. Structural priming: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 427–459. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scheepers C. 2003. Syntactic priming of relative clause attachments: persistence of structural configuration in sentence production. Cognition 89, 179–205. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shen, Y., He Y., & Gu. Y. 2001. 生成语法理论与汉语语法研究 [Generative Grammar and Chinese Grammar Research]. Harbin: Harbin Educational Press.Google Scholar
Tang, T.-C. 1978. Double object constructions in Chinese. In Proceedings of Symposium on Chinese Linguistics, Linguistics Institute of the Linguistics Society of America 1977, Robert L. Cheng, Li Ying-Che & Ting-Chi Tang (eds.), 67–96. Taipei: Student.Google Scholar
Ting, J., & Chang M. 2004. The category of gei in Mandarin Chinese and grammaticalization. Taiwan Journal of Linguistics, 2 (2), 45–74.Google Scholar
Zhang, N. 1998. Argument interpretation in the ditransitive construction. Nordic Journal of Linguistics 21, 179–209. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (1)

Cited by one other publication

Li, Yi, Benedikt Szmrecsanyi & Weiwei Zhang
2024. Across time, space, and genres: measuring probabilistic grammar distances between varieties of Mandarin. Linguistics Vanguard DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 15 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.