Article published In:
Language Impairment in Bilingual Children: State of the art 2017
Edited by Theodoros Marinis, Sharon Armon-Lotem and George Pontikas
[Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 7:3/4] 2017
► pp. 431459
References (100)
References
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4 th ed. (DSM-IV). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (2003). Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36(3), 189–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 81, 47–89. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, 70(3), 636–644. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2001). Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006). Effect of bilingualism and computer video game experience on the Simon task. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 60(1), 68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Grady, C., Chau, W., Ishii, R., Gunji, A., & Pantev, C. (2005). Effect of bilingualism on cognitive control in the Simon task: Evidence from MEG. NeuroImage, 24(1), 40–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and aging, 19(2), 290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Viswanathan, M. (2009). Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition, 112(3), 494–500. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1992). The underlying nature of specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(1), 3–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1994). Grammatical errors in specific language impairment: Competence or performance limitations?. Applied Psycholinguistics, 15(4), 507–550. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2005). Executive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symptomatology I: Generativity. Autism, 9(1), 7–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (2014). Ten questions about terminology for children with unexplained language problems. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 49(4), 381–415. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blasi, G., Goldberg, T. E., Weickert, T., Das, S., Kohn, P., Zoltick, B., Bertolino, A., Callicott, J.H., Weinberger, D.R., & Mattay, V. S. (2006). Brain regions underlying response inhibition and interference monitoring and suppression. European Journal of Neuroscience, 23(6), 1658–1664. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bonifacci, P., Giombini, L., Bellocchi, S., & Contento, S. (2011). Speed of processing, anticipation, inhibition and working memory in bilinguals. Developmental Science, 14(2), 256–269. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bunge, S. A., Dudukovic, N. M., Thomason, M. E., Vaidya, C. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children: Evidence from fMRI. Neuron, 33(2), 301–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cardy, J. E. O., Tannock, R., Johnson, A. M., & Johnson, C. J. (2010). The contribution of processing impairments to SLI: Insights from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Communication Disorders, 43(2), 77–91. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11(2), 282–298. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chapman, R. S. (1997). Language development in children and adolescents with Down syndrome. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 3(4), 307–312. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chevrie-Muller, C., & Plaza, M. (2001). N-EEL: Nouvelles épreuves pour l’examen du langage [New tasks for the assemsent of language]. Paris: Éditions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée (ECPA).Google Scholar
Conti‐Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B. (2001). Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(6), 741–748. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106(1), 59–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crago, M., & Paradis, J. (2003). Two of a kind? Commonalities and variation in languages and language learners. In Y. Levy & J. Schaeffer (Eds.), Language competence across populations: Towards a definition of specific language impairment (pp.97–110). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Crutchley, A. (1999). Bilingual children with SLI attending language units: getting the bigger picture. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 15(3), 201–217. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principals of frontal lobe function (pp.466–503). London: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Bruin, A., Treccani, B., & Della Sala, S. (2014). Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: An example of publication bias? Psychological Science, 26(1), 99–107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deltour, J. J. (1992). Test de closure grammaticale (TCG) [Test of grammatical closure (TCG)]. Liège: Presse Universitaire de Liège.Google Scholar
Deltour, J. J., & Hupkens, D. (1980). TVAP, Test de vocabulaire actif et passif [TVAP, Test of active and passive vocabulary]. Paris: Editions EAP.Google Scholar
Dollaghan, C., & Campbell, T. F. (1998). Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41(5), 1136–1146. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Thériault-Whalen, C. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1993). Echelle de vocabulaire en images peabody: Adaptation française du Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test [Scale of vocabulary using peabody pictures : French adaptation of the peabody picture vocabulary test]. Toronto, Canada: Psycan.Google Scholar
Elin Thordardottir, E. T. (2015). Proposed diagnostic procedures for use in bilingual and cross-linguistic contexts. In S. Armon-Lotem, J. de Jong, & N. Meir (Eds.), Methods for assessing multilingual children: Disentangling bilingualism from Specific Language Impairment (pp.331–358). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Elin Thordardottir, E. T., & Brandeker, M. (2013). The effect of bilingual exposure versus language impairment on nonword repetition and sentence imitation scores. Journal of Communication Disorders, 46(1), 1–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Engel de Abreu, P. M. E., Cruz-Santos, A., Tourinho, C. J., Martin, R., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingualism enriches the poor enhanced cognitive control in low-income minority children. Psychological Science, 23(11), 1364–1371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Engel de Abreu, P.M.E., Cruz-Santos, A., & Puglisi, M. (2014). Specific language impairment in language‐minority children from low‐income families. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 49(6), 736–747. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Epstein, B., Shafer, V. L., Melara, R. D., & Schwartz, R. G. (2014). Can children with SLI detect cognitive conflict? Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(4), 1453–1467. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(1), 101. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greenslade, K. J., Plante, E., & Vance, R. (2009). The diagnostic accuracy and construct validity of the Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test Preschool. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 40(2), 150–160. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grüter, T. (2005). Comprehension and production of French object clitics by child second language learners and children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26(3), 363–91. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Henry, L. A., Messer, D. J., & Nash, G. (2012). Executive functioning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(1), 37–45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26(1), 55–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, L. M., & Gillam, R. B. (2004). Verbal and spatial information processing constraints in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(1), 114–125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huizinga, M., Dolan, C. V., & Van der Molen, M. W. (2006). Age-related change in executive function: Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2017–2036. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iluz-Cohen, P., & Armon-Lotem, S. (2013). Language proficiency and executive control in bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(4), 884–899. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Im-Bolter, N., Johnson J., and Pascual Leone, J. (2006). Processing limitations in children with SLI: The role of executive function. Child Development, 77(6), 1822–1841. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jensen de Lopez, K. M., & Baker, A. E. (2015). Executive functions in the assessment of bilingual children with language impairment. In S. Armon-Lotem, J. de Jong, & N. Meir (Eds.), Methods for assessing multilingual children: Disentangling bilingualism from Specific Language Impairment (pp.277–297). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Joanisse, M. F., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1998). Specific language impairment: A deficit in grammar or processing? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(7), 240–247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003). Phonology and syntax in specific language impairment: Evidence from a connectionist model. Brain and Language, 86(1), 40–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kail, R. (1994). A method for studying the generalized slowing hypothesis in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 37(2), 418–421. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kapa, L. L., & Plante, E. (2015). Executive Function in SLI: Recent Advances and Future Directions. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 2(3), 245–252. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). K-ABC-II. Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children. Second edition. Manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Khomsi, A. (2000). Évaluation du langage oral: ELO [Assessment of spoken language : ELO]. Paris: Éditions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée (ECPA).Google Scholar
Klenberg, L., Korkman, M., & Lahti-Nuuttila, P. (2001). Differential development of attention and executive functions in 3- to 12-year-old Finnish children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(1), 407–428. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kohnert, K. (2010). Bilingual children with primary language impairment: Issues, evidence and implications for clinical actions. Journal of Communication Disorders, 43(6), 456–473. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kohnert, K., Windsor, J., & Yim, D. (2006). Do language-based processing tasks separate children with language impairment from typical bilinguals? Learning Disabilities: Research & Practice, 21(1), 19–29.Google Scholar
Kindlon, D., Mezzacappa, E., & Earls, F. (1995). Psychometric properties of impulsivity measures: Temporal stability, validity and factor structure. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 361, 645–661. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Dijkstra, A. (2002). The bilingual lexicon. In R. Kaplan (Ed.), Handbook of Applied Linguistics (pp. 301–321). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lakshmanan, U. (2012). Child second language acquisition. In P. Robinson (Ed.), The Routledge Encyclopedia of Second Language Acquisition (pp.71–75). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Lecocq, P. (1996). L'ÉCOSSE, une épreuve de compréhension syntaxico-sémantique : Adaptation française du Test of Reception of Grammar (TROG) [The ECOSSE, a task of syntactic and semantic understanding : French adaptation of the Test of Reception of Grammar (TROG)]. Paris: Presses Universitaires Septentrion.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. B. (2014). Children with specific language impairment. Second edition. Cambridge: MIT press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Logan, G. D., Schachar, R. J., & Tannock, R. (1997). Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychological Science, 8(1), 60–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., Anderson, J. A., Craik, F. I., Grady, C., & Bialystok, E. (2010). Distinct neural correlates for two types of inhibition in bilinguals: Response inhibition versus interference suppression. Brain and Cognition, 74(3), 347–357. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(1), 81–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marton, K., Kelmenson, L., & Pinkhasova, M. (2007). Inhibition control and working memory capacity in children with SLI. Psikhologyah: ketav'et mada'i Yisre'eli le-'iyun ule-mehkar, 50(2), 110.Google Scholar
Marton, K., Campanelli, L., Scheuer, J., Yoon, J., & Eichorn, N. (2012). Executive function profiles in children with and without specific language impairment. Rivista di psicolinguistica applicata, 12(3), 57.Google Scholar
McGregor, K. K., Newman, R. M., Reilly, R. M., & Capone, N. C. (2002). Semantic representation and naming in children with speci c language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 45(5), 998–1014. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Messer, D., & Dockrell, J. E. (2006). Children’s naming and word- finding difficulties: Descriptions and explanations. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(2), 309–324. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Messer, M. H., Leseman, P. P., Boom, J., & Mayo, A. Y. (2010). Phonotactic probability effect in nonword recall and its relationship with vocabulary in monolingual and bilingual preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105(4), 306–323. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mezzacappa, E. (2004). Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: Developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development, 75(5), 1373–1386. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miles, J., & Shevlin, M. (2001). Applying regression and correlation: A guide for students and researchers. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, J. W. (2000). Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(2), 293–308. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, J. W., Magimairaj, B. M., & Finney, M. C. (2010). Working memory and specific language impairment: An update on the relation and perspectives on assessment and treatment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology: 19(1), 78–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological bulletin, 126(2), 220. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noterdaeme, M., Amorosa, H., Mildenberger, K., Sitter, S., & Minow, F. (2001). Evaluation of attention problems in children with autism and children with a specific language disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 10(1), 58–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. (2004). The relevance of specific language impairment in understanding the role of transfer in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25(1), 67–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Crago, M. (2000). Tense and temporality: Similarities and differences between language-impaired and second-language children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(4), 834–848. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2004). Comparing L2 and SLI grammars in child French: Focus on DP. In P. Prévost & J. Paradis (Eds.), The acquisition of French in different contexts: Focus on functional categories (pp.89–107). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Rice, M. L., Crago, M., & Marquis, J. (2008). The acquisition of tense in English: Distinguishing child second language from first language and specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29(4), 689–722. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, S. C., Lewedeg, V., & Oller, D. K. (1997). The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18(1), 41–58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(1), 51–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pierart, B., Comblain, A., Grégoire, J., Mousty, P., & Noël, M. P. (1997). ISADYLE: Instrument pour le screening et l'examen approfondi des dysfonctionnements du langage chez l'enfant [ISADYLE : Instrument for the screening and the in-depth assesment of child language dysfunctions]. In Meeting annuel de la Société Belge de Psychologie.Google Scholar
Plante, E., & Vance, R. (1994). Selection of preschool language tests: A data-based approach. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 25(1), 15–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1985). Raven’s progressive matrices. A manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. London: H. K. Lewis.Google Scholar
Roello, M., Ferretti, M. L., Colonnello, V., & Levi, G. (2015). When words lead to solutions: Executive function deficits in preschool children with specific language impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 371, 216–222. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rowe, J., Lavender, A., & Turk, V. (2006). Cognitive executive function in Down’s syndrome. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45(1), 5–17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rice, M. L., & Wexler, K. (1996). A phenotype of specific language impairment: Extended optional infinitives. In M. L. Rice (Ed.), Toward a genetics of language (pp.215–237). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Russell, J., Jarrold, C., & Hood, B. (1999). Two intact executive capacities in children with autism: Implications for the core executive dysfunctions in the disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(2), 103–112. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, R. G. (2009). Specific language impairment. In R.G. Schwartz (Ed.), Handbook of Child Language Disorders (pp.3–43). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Spaulding, T. J. (2010). Investigating mechanisms of suppression in preschool children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(3), 725–738. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tannock, R., & Schachar, R. (1996). Executive dysfunction as an underlying mechanism of behavior and language problems in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. In J. H. Beitchman, N. J. Cohen, M. M. Konstantareas & R. Tannock (Eds.), Language, learning and behavioral disorders. Developmental, biological and clinical perspectives (pp.128–155). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of the input in the acquisition of third person singular verbs in English. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46(4), 863–877. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thorn, A. S. C., & Gathercole, S.E. (1999). Language-specific knowledge and short-term memory in bilingual and non-bilingual children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52(2), 303–324. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40(6), 1245–1260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vugs, B., Hendriks, M., Cuperus, J., & Verhoeven, L. (2014). Working memory performance and executive function behaviors in young children with SLI. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(1), 62–74. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1989). Wechsler preschool and primary scales of intelligence. New York, NY: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wilson, S. P., & Kipp, K. (1998). The development of efficient inhibition: Evidence from directed-forgetting tasks. Developmental Review, 18(1), 86–123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Windsor, J., Kohnert, K., Lobitz, K. F., Pham, G. T. (2010). Cross-language non-word repetition by bilingual and monolingual children. American Journal of Speech- Language Pathology, 19(4), 298–310. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
World Health Organization. (1993). The ICD-10 classification for mental and behavioral disorders: Diagnostic criteria for research. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
Cited by (15)

Cited by 15 other publications

Gandolfi, Elena, Giovanna Diotallevi & Paola Viterbori
2024. Morphological and Inhibitory Skills in Monolingual and Bilingual Children With and Without Developmental Language Disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 67:8  pp. 2620 ff. DOI logo
Niu, Tengfei, Shiqiang Wang, Jingxi Ma, Xiaoping Zeng & Ruiling Xue
2024. Executive functions in children with developmental language disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Frontiers in Neuroscience 18 DOI logo
Joyce, Teresa, Serena Vanzan, Nichola Stuart & Anna Barnett
2023. Inhibition skills in children with developmental coordination disorder. Developmental Neuropsychology 48:4  pp. 147 ff. DOI logo
Andreou, Maria, Eleni Peristeri & Ianthi Maria Tsimpli
2022. Reference maintenance in the narratives of Albanian–Greek and Russian–Greek children with Developmental Language Disorder: A study on crosslinguistic effects. First Language 42:2  pp. 292 ff. DOI logo
Hadjadj, Olivia, Margaret Kehoe & Hélène Delage
2022. Dynamic Assessment Identifies Morphosyntactic Deficits in Mono- and Bilingual Children with Developmental Language Disorder. Languages 7:4  pp. 295 ff. DOI logo
Aguilar-Mediavilla, Eva, Jorge Luis Guirado-Moreno, Víctor Sánchez-Azanza, Daniel Adrover-Roig & Mario Valera-Pozo
2021. Intervención en el control inhibitorio en niños con y sin trastorno de lenguaje dentro del aula. Revista de Investigación en Logopedia 11:Especial  pp. 115 ff. DOI logo
Lowe, Cassandra J., Isu Cho, Samantha F. Goldsmith & J. Bruce Morton
2021. The Bilingual Advantage in Children’s Executive Functioning Is Not Related to Language Status: A Meta-Analytic Review. Psychological Science 32:7  pp. 1115 ff. DOI logo
Blom, Elma & Tessel Boerma
2020. Do Children With Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) Have Difficulties With Interference Control, Visuospatial Working Memory, and Selective Attention? Developmental Patterns and the Role of Severity and Persistence of DLD. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 63:9  pp. 3036 ff. DOI logo
Boerma, Tessel & Elma Blom
2020. Effects of developmental language disorder and bilingualism on children’s executive functioning: A longitudinal study. Research in Developmental Disabilities 107  pp. 103782 ff. DOI logo
De Cat, Cécile
2020. PREDICTING LANGUAGE PROFICIENCY IN BILINGUAL CHILDREN. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 42:2  pp. 279 ff. DOI logo
Gandolfi, Elena & Paola Viterbori
2020. Inhibitory Control Skills and Language Acquisition in Toddlers and Preschool Children. Language Learning 70:3  pp. 604 ff. DOI logo
Joyce, Teresa, Serena Vanzan, Nichola Stuart & Anna L. Barnett
2020. Motor and inhibition skills in children with and without developmental co-ordination disorder (DCD). The Cognitive Psychology Bulletin 1:5  pp. 32 ff. DOI logo
Peristeri, Eleni, Maria Andreou, Ianthi Maria Tsimpli & Stephanie Durrleman
Yim, Dongsun, Jiyun Han, Eun Song, Sookyung Lee & Jia Han
2020. A Comparative Study on the Vocabulary Learning of Children According to Selective Attention Latent Group. Communication Sciences & Disorders 25:1  pp. 26 ff. DOI logo
Marton, Klara, Zsuzsanna Kovi & Tímea Egri
2018. Is interference control in children with specific language impairment similar to that of children with autistic spectrum disorder?. Research in Developmental Disabilities 72  pp. 179 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 21 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.