The current study assessed independent and combined effects of SLI and bilingualism on tasks tapping into verbal short-term memory (vSTM) with varying linguistic load in two languages (Russian and Hebrew). The study explored the extent to which the presence of SLI is related to limited vSTM storage and bilingualism is associated with reduced vocabulary size.
A total of 190 monolingual and bilingual children aged 5;5–6;8 participated in the current study: 108 sequential Russian-Hebrew bilinguals (18 with SLI), 48 Hebrew monolinguals (13 with SLI) and 34 Russian monolinguals (14 with SLI). Children performed three repetition tasks: forward-digit span (FWD), non-word repetition (NWR) and sentence repetition (SRep); bilingual children were tested in both of their languages.
Results indicated a negative effect of SLI on all experimental tasks tapping into vSTM. The effect of SLI rose as a function of increased linguistic load. Regarding bilingualism, no effect was found on the measure of vSTM with the lowest linguistic load (FWD), while its effect was robust once the linguistic load was increased (SRep). The results reported in this study bring evidence that lower performance on measures of vSTM in children with SLI and bilingual children stem from different sources. Although, children with SLI have limitations of vSTM, deficient vSTM cannot fully account for the linguistic difficulties observed in children with SLI. As for bilingualism, it does not affect verbal storage when the linguistic load is minimal, while poor performance in bilingual children on tasks with greater linguistic load is attributed to smaller vocabulary sizes.
Alloway, T. P., & Gathercole, S. E. (2005a). The role of sentence recall in reading and language skills of children with learning difficulties. Learning and Individual Differences, 151(41), 271–282.
Alloway, T. P., & Gathercole, S. E. (2005b). Working memory and short-term sentence recall in young children. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17(2), 207–220.
Altman, C., Harel, E., Meir, N., Iluz-Cohen, P., Walters, J., & Armon-Lotem, S. (2014). Can the Goralnik (1995) be used to screen bilingual children? Focus on Accuracy and Specificity. Presented at the conference on multilingualism and multi-culturalism in communication disorders. Hadassah Academic College.
Antoniou, M., Liang, E., Ettlinger, M., & Wong, P. C. M. (2015). The bilingual advantage in phonetic learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(4), 683–695.
Archibald, L. M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Short-term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41(6), 675–693.
Armon-Lotem, S., & Chiat, S. (2012). How do sequential bilingual children perform on non-word repetition tasks? In A. K. Biller, E. Y. Chung, & A. E. Kimball (Eds.), Proceedings of the 36th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. (pp. 53–62). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Armon-Lotem, S., & Meir, N. (2016). Diagnostic accuracy of repetition tasks for the identification of specific language impairment (SLI) in bilingual children: evidence from Russian and Hebrew. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders. Published online before print.
Baddeley, A., Vallar, G., & Wilson, B. (1987). Sentence comprehension and phonological memory: Some neuropsychological evidence. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and Performance XII (pp. 509–529). London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Baddeley, A. (2001). Is Working Memory still working?American Psychologist, 56(11), 851–864.
Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S. E., & Papagno, C. (1998). The Phonological Loop as a Language Learning Device. Psychological Review, 105(1), 158–173.
Bialystok, E., & Shapero, D. (2005). Ambiguous benefits: The effect of bilingualism on reversing ambiguous figures. Developmental Science, 8(6), 595–604.
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K. F., & Yang, S. (2010). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(4), 525–531.
Bishop, D. V. (1992). The Underlying Nature of Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(1), 3–66.
Buac, M., Gross, M., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2016). Predictors of processing-based task performance in bilingual and monolingual children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 62(0), 12–29.
Chiat, S. (2015). Nonword Repetition. In S. Armon-Lotem, J. de Jong, & N. Meir (Eds.), Assessing Multilingual Children: Disentangling Bilingualism from Language Impairment (pp. 125–150). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Chiat, S., Armon-Lotem, S., Marinis, T., Polišenská, K., Roy, P., & Seeff-Gabriel, B. (2013). Chiat, Armon-Lotem, Marinis, Polisenska, Roy, & Seeff-Gabriel (2013). In V. Mueller Gathercole (Ed.), Issues in the Assessment of Bilinguals (pp. 56–89). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Chondrogianni, V., Marinis,T., Edwards, S., & Blom, E. (2015). Production and on-line comprehension of definite articles and clitic pronouns by Greek sequential bilingual children and monolingual children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(5), 1155–1191.
Cohen, J. (1969). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. New York: Academic Press.
Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B. (2001). Psycholinguistic Markers for Specific Language Impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(6), 741–748.
Engel de Abreu, P. M. (2011). Working memory in multilingual children: Is there a bilingual effect?Memory, 19(5), 529–537.
Engel de Abreu, P. M., Baldassi, M., Puglisi, M. L., & Befi-Lopes, D. M. (2013). Cross-Linguistic and Cross-Cultural Effects on Verbal Working Memory and Vocabulary: Testing Language Minority Children with an Immigrant Background. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 56(2), 630–642.
Gagarina, N., Klassert, A., & Topaj, N. (2010). Russian language proficiency test for multilingual children. ZAS Papers in Linguistics 541. Sonderheft. Berlin: ZAS.
Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Is nonword repetition a test of phonological memory or long-term knowledge? It all depends on the nonwords. Memory & Cognition, 23(1), 83–94.
Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Hall, M., & Peaker, S. M. (2001). Dissociable lexical and phonological inttuences on serial recognition and serial recall. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A(1), 1–30.
Gathercole, S. E., Tiffany, C., Briscoe, J., Thorn, A., & The ALSPAC team. (2004). Developmental consequences of poor phonological short-term memory function in childhood: a longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(0), 1–14.
Gathercole, S., & Baddeley, A. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection?Journal of Memory and Language, 29(3), 336–360.
Goralnik, E. (1995 ). Goralnik Screening Test for Hebrew. Even Yehuda: Matan.
Graf Estes, K., Evans, J. L., & Else-Quest, N. M. (2007). Differences in the nonword repetition performance of children with and without specific language impairment: a meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(1), 177–195.
Gray, S. (2003). Diagnostic accuracy and test-retest reliability of nonword repetition and digit span tasks administered to preschool children with specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36(2), 129–151.
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., & Simon-Cereijido, G. (2010). Using Nonword Repetition Tasks for the Identification of Language Impairment in Spanish-English-Speaking Children: Does the Language of Assessment Matter?Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 25(1), 48–58.
Iluz-Cohen, P., & Armon-Lotem, S. (2013). Language proficiency and executive control in bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(4), 884–899.
Kogan, N. (2011). Monolingual and Bilingual Children’s Acquisition of Basic Linguistic Prerequisites of Reading. (Unublished PhD thesis. Ramat-Gan: Bar-Ilan University).
Laloi, A. (2015). Language and executive functioning in the context of specific language impairment and bilingualism. PhD thesis. Amsterdam: University of Amsterdam.
Leonard, L. B. (2014). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press.
Leonard, L. B., Dromi, E., & Adam, G. (2000). Tense and finiteness in the speech of children with specic language impairment acquiring Hebrew, International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 351(31), 319–335.
Marinis, T., & Armon-Lotem, S. (2015). Sentence Repetition. In S. Armon-Lotem, J. de Jong, & N. Meir (Eds.), Assessing Multilingual Children: Disentangling bilingualism from language impairment (pp. 95–124). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Marshall, C. R., & van der Lely, H. K. (2009). Effects of Word Position and Stress on Onset Cluster Production: Evidence from Typical Development, Specific Language Impairment, and Dyslexia. Language, 85(1), 39–57.
Meir, N., & Amon-Lotem, S. (2015). Disentangling bilingualism from SLI in Heritage Russian: The impact of L2 properties and length of exposure to the L2. In C. Hamann & E. Ruigendijk (Eds.). Language Acquisition and Development: Proceedings of GALA 2013 (pp. 299–314). Newcastle upon Tyne, UK: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
Meir, N., Walters, J., & Armon-Lotem, S. (2016). Disentangling SLI and bilingualism using Sentence Repetition Tasks: the impact of L1 and TL properties. International Journal of Bilingualism, 20(4), 421–452.
Messer, M. H., Leseman, P. P., Boom, J., & Mayo, A. (2010). Phonotactic probability effect in nonword recall and its relationship with vocabulary in monolingual and bilingual preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105(4), 306–323.
Metsala, J. L. (1999). Young children’s phonological awareness and nonword repetition as a function of vocabulary development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(1), 3–19.
Mor-Sommerfeld, A., Azaiza, F., & Hertz-Lazarowitz, R., (2007). Into the future: towards bilingual education in Israel. Education, Citizenship And Social Justice, 2(1), 5–22.
Paradis, J. (2010). The interface between bilingual development and specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31(2), 227–252.
Pavlenko, A. (2008). Multilingualism in Post-Soviet Countries: Language Revival, Language Removal, and Sociolinguistic Theory. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 11(3–4), 275–314.
Polišenská, K., Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2015). Sentence repetition: What does the task measure?International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 50(1), 106–118.
Raven, J. (1998). Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Rice, M., & Wexler, K. (1996). Toward Tense as a Clinical Marker of Specific Language Impairment in English-Speaking Children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 39(6), 1239–1257.
Richardson, J. T. E. (2007). Measures of short-term memory: A historical review. Cortex, 43(5), 635–650.
Richardson, J. T. E. (2011). Eta squared and partial eta squared as measures of effect size in educational research. Educational Research Review, 6(2), 135–147.
Riches, N. G. (2012). Sentence repetition in children with specific language impairment: an investigation of underlying mechanisms. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 47(5), 499–510.
Rispens, J., & Baker, A. (2012). Nonword Repetition: The Relative Contributions of Phonological Short-Term Memory and Phonological Representations in Children with Language and Reading Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55(3), 683–694.
Roy, P., & Chiat, S. (2004). A Prosodically Controlled Word and Nonword Repetition Task for 2- to 4-Year-Olds: Evidence From Typically Developing Children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(1), 223–234.
Sharp, K. M., & Gathercole, V. C. (2013). Can a novel word repetition task be a language-neutral assessment tool? Evidence from Welsh -English bilingual children. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 29(1), 77–89.
Smolík, F., & Vávru, P. (2014). Sentence imitation as a marker of SLI in Czech: disproportionate impairment of verbs and clitics. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(3), 837–849.
Summers, C., Bohman, T. M., Gillam, R. B., Bedore, L. M., & Peña, E. D. (2010). Bilingual performance on nonword repetition in Spanish and English. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 45(4), 480–493.
Thordardottir, E., (2015). Proposed Diagnostic Procedures for Use in Bilingual and Cross-Linguistic Contexts. In S. Armon-Lotem, J. de Jong, & N. Meir (Eds.), Assessing Multilingual Children: Disentangling Bilingualism from Language Impairmen (pp. 331–358). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Thordardottir, E., & Brandeker, M. (2013). The effect of bilingual exposure versus language impairment on nonword repetition and sentence imitation scores. Journal of Communication Disorders, 46(1), 1–16.
Thordardottir, E., Kehayia, E., Mazer, B., Lessard, N., Majnemer, A., Sutton, A., Trudeau, N., & Chilingaryan, G. (2011). Sensitivity and specificity of French language and processing measures for the identification of primary language impairment at age 5. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 54(2), 580–597.
Thorn, A. S., & Gathercole, S. E. (2001). Language differences in verbal short-term memory do not exclusively originate in the process of subvocal rehearsal. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8(2), 357–364.
van der Lely, H. K. J., Rosen, S., & Adlard, A. (2004). Grammatical language impairment and the specificity of cognitive domains: Relations between auditory and language abilities. Cognition, 94(2), 167–183.
Verhoeven, L., Steenge, J., & van Balkom, H. (2012). Linguistic transfer in bilingual children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Communication Disorders, 47(2), 76–183.
Wechsler, D. (1998). Wechsler Scales of Intelligence-R 95: Hebrew Version. Jerusalem, Israel: Ministry of Education; 1998.
Willis, C. S., & Gathercole, S. E. (2001). Phonological short-term memory contributions to sentence processing in young children. Memory, 9(4–6), 49–363.
Windsor, J., Kohnert, K., Lobitz, K. F., & Pham, G. T. (2010). Cross-language nonword repetition by bilingual and monolingual children. American Jornal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19(4), 298–310.
Ziethe, A., Eysholdt, U., & Doellinger, M. (2013). Sentence repetition and digit span: Potential markers of bilingual children with suspected SLI?Logopedics Phoniatrics Vocology, 38(1), 1–10.
Cited by (17)
Cited by 17 other publications
Fu, Nga Ching, Si Chen, Kamila Polišenská, Angel Chan, Rachel Kan & Shula Chiat
2024. Nonword Repetition in Children With Developmental Language Disorder: Revisiting the Case of Cantonese. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 67:6 ► pp. 1772 ff.
Yaari, Hadar, Sveta Fichman, Pola Osher, Fedor Dorokhov & Carmit Altman
2024. Disfluencies as a window to macrostructure performance in the narrative of bilingual children. Ampersand 13 ► pp. 100187 ff.
Bihovsky, Alina, Michal Ben-Shachar & Natalia Meir
2023. Language abilities, not cognitive control, predict language mixing behavior in bilingual speakers with aphasia. Journal of Communication Disorders 105 ► pp. 106367 ff.
Bihovsky, Alina, Michal Ben-Shachar & Natalia Meir
2024. Semantic Feature Analysis (SFA) treatment in L1 and L2 in bilingual aphasia: Effects of cognitive and language factors. Aphasiology 38:4 ► pp. 683 ff.
Gilhuber, Christina Sophia, Tracy Jane Raulston & Kasie Galley
2023. Language and communication skills in multilingual children on the autism spectrum: A systematic review. Autism 27:6 ► pp. 1516 ff.
Wimmer, Eva & Anna-Lena Scherger
2022. Working Memory Skills in DLD: Does Bilingualism Make a Difference?. Languages 7:4 ► pp. 287 ff.
Pratt, Amy S., Elizabeth D. Peña & Lisa M. Bedore
2021. Sentence repetition with bilinguals with and without DLD: Differential effects of memory, vocabulary, and exposure. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 24:2 ► pp. 305 ff.
Hamann, Cornelia, Solveig Chilla, Lina Abed Ibrahim & István Fekete
2020. Language assessment tools for Arabic-speaking heritage and refugee children in Germany. Applied Psycholinguistics 41:6 ► pp. 1375 ff.
Meir, Natalia & Rama Novogrodsky
2020. Syntactic abilities and verbal memory in monolingual and bilingual children with High Functioning Autism (HFA). First Language 40:4 ► pp. 341 ff.
MEIR, Natalia & Rama NOVOGRODSKY
2023. Referential expressions in monolingual and bilingual children with and without Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD): A study of informativeness and definiteness. Journal of Child Language 50:2 ► pp. 215 ff.
Novogrodsky, Rama & Natalia Meir
2020. Multilingual Children with Special Needs in Early Education. In Handbook of Early Language Education [Springer International Handbooks of Education, ], ► pp. 1 ff.
Novogrodsky, Rama & Natalia Meir
2022. Multilingual Children with Special Needs in Early Education. In Handbook of Early Language Education [Springer International Handbooks of Education, ], ► pp. 669 ff.
Talli, Ioanna & Stavroula Stavrakaki
2020. Short-term memory, working memory and linguistic abilities in bilingual children with Developmental Language Disorder. First Language 40:4 ► pp. 437 ff.
Zebib, Racha, Laurice Tuller, Cornelia Hamann, Lina Abed Ibrahim & Philippe Prévost
2020. Syntactic complexity and verbal working memory in bilingual children with and without Developmental Language Disorder. First Language 40:4 ► pp. 461 ff.
Degani, Tamar, Varda Kreiser & Rama Novogrodsky
2019. The joint effects of bilingualism, DLD and item frequency on children's lexical‐retrieval performance. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 54:3 ► pp. 485 ff.
Meir, Natalia
2018. Morpho-Syntactic Abilities of Unbalanced Bilingual Children: A Closer Look at the Weaker Language. Frontiers in Psychology 9
Meir, Natalia & Sharon Armon-Lotem
2017. Independent and Combined Effects of Socioeconomic Status (SES) and Bilingualism on Children’s Vocabulary and Verbal Short-Term Memory. Frontiers in Psychology 8
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 21 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.