Effects of education on the production of plural morphology among
bilingual speakers of Yucatec Maya and Spanish
Yucatec Maya differs from many better-known languages in that it has optional
plural marking. In a psycholinguistic study of the production of optional plural
marking with college-enrolled speakers of Yucatec Maya,
Butler, Jaeger, and Bohnemeyer (2014) found that
conceptual number information influences the production of optional plural
marking. Since the participants in the
Butler et al. (2014) study are not necessarily representative of
speakers of Yucatec Maya, we examine the effects of conceptual number
information, via the manipulation of set size, while factoring in the effects of
age, education and language use variables on the production of optional plural
morphology among bilingual speakers of Yucatec Maya and Spanish speaking in
Yucatec Maya. In addition to finding effects of conceptual information, we found
that education, but not age, significantly influences the production of plural
morphology in Yucatec Maya. Participants with higher levels of education were
more sensitive to conceptual number information.
Article outline
- 1.Introduction
- 1.1Production of number marking and agreement
- 1.2Acquisition of number marking
- 1.3Bilingual sentence processing
- 1.4Bilingualism in the Yucatan Peninsula
- 1.5Previous findings, goals and predictions of present study
- 2.Methods
- 2.1Participants
- 2.2Procedure
- 2.3Materials
- 2.4Coding and exclusions
- 3.Analysis
- 3.1Set size
- 3.2Education and age
- 3.3Language use variables
- 3.4Matching plural agreement
- 3.5Interaction of set size and level of education
- 4.Discussion
- 4.1Conceptual and grammatical number information
- 4.2Role of education
- 4.3Conclusions
- Acknowledgments
- Notes
- Appendices
- Yucatec Maya number marking and agreement
-
References
References (78)
References
Acuña-Fariña, J. C. (2009). The
linguistics and psycholinguistics of agreement: A tutorial
overview. Lingua, 1191, 389–424.
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects
modeling with crossed random effects mixed effects modeling with crossed
random effects for subjects and
items. Journal of Memory and
Language, 591, 390–412.
Barner, D., Lui, T., & Zapf, J. (2012). Is
two a plural marker in early child
language? Developmental
Psychology, 48(1), 10–17.
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015a). Fitting
linear mixed-effects models using
lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, ArXiv
e-print.
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015b). lme4:
Linear mixed-effects models using eigen and s4 (R
Package version 1.1–9).
Berg, T. (1998). The
resolution of number agreement conflicts in English and German agreement
patterns. Linguistics, 361, 41–70.
Blaha Pfeiler, B., & Carrillo Carreón, C. (2001). La
adquisición del maya yucateco: el
número. In C. R. y Lourdes de León Pasquel (Ed.), La
adquisición de la lengua materna. Espaüol, lenguas Mayas,
Euskera (pp. 75–97). México: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México/Centro de Investigaciones y Estudios Superiores en Antropología Social.
Bock, J. K., & Cutting, J. C. (1992). Regulating
mental energy: Performance units in language
production. Journal of Memory and
Language, 311, 99–127.
Bock, J. K., & Eberhard, K. M. (1993). Meaning,
sound and syntax in English number
agreement. Language and Cognitive
Processes, 81, 57–99.
Bock, J. K., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1994). Language
production: grammatical
encoding. In M. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook
of
Psycholinguistics (pp. 945–984). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Bock, J. K., & Miller, C. A. (1991). Broken
agreement. Cognitive
Psychology, 231, 45–93.
Bohnemeyer, J. (2002). The
grammar of time reference in Yukatek
Maya. Muenchen: Lincom Europa.
Bohnemeyer, J. (2009). Linking
without grammatical relations in Yucatec: Alignment, extraction and
control. In Y. Nishina, Y. M. Shin, S. Skopeteas, E. Verhoeven, & J. Helmbrecht (Eds.), Issues
in functional-typological linguistics and language theory: A festschrift for
Christian Lehmann on the occasion of his 60th
birthday (pp. 185–214). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Bohnemeyer, J., Butler, L. K., & Jaeger, T. F. (2015). Head-marking
and agreement: Evidence from Yucatec
Maya. In J. Fleischhauer, A. Latrouite, & R. Osswald (Eds.), Explorations
of the syntax-semantics
interface. Düselforf: Düselforf University Press.
Borer, H. (2005). Structuring
Sense. In Name
Only (Vol. I1). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Brainshark,
Inc. (2014). Slideshark. [URL].
Branigan, H., Pickering, M., & Tanaka, M. (2008). Contributions
of animacy to grammatical function assignment and word order during
production. Lingua, 118(2), 172–189.
Breslow, N. E., & Clayton, D. G. (1993). Approximate
inference in generalized linear mixed
models. Journal of the American Statistical
Association, 88(421), 9–25.
Bricker, V. R. (1981). The
source of the ergative split in Yucatec
Maya. Journal of Mayan
Linguistics, 21, 83–127.
Brown-Schmidt, S., & Konopka, A. E. (2008). Little
houses and casas pequeüas: Message formulation and
syntactic form in unscripted speech with speakers of English and
Spanish. Cognition, 109(2), 274–280.
Butler, L. K. (2011). The
morphosyntax and processing of number marking in Yucatec
Maya (Unpublished doctoral
dissertation). University of Arizona.
Butler, L. K., Jaeger, T. F., & Bohnemeyer, J. (2014). Syntactic
constraints and production preferences for optional plural marking in
Yucatec
Maya. In A. Machicao y Priemer, A. Nolda, & A. Sioupi (Eds.), Zwischen
kern und peripherie (Studia
Grammatica) (Vol. 751). Berlin: Akadmie-Verlag.
CONAFE. (2000). Escuela
y comunidades originarias en México. Consejo Nacional del Fomento Educativo. Mexico.
Corbett, G. G. (2000). Number. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
den Dikken, M. (2001). “Pluringulars”,
pronouns and quirky agreement. The Linguistic
Review, 181, 19–41.
Desmet, T., & Duyck, W. (2007). Bilingual
language processing. Language and Linguistics
Compass, 1/31, 168–194.
Eberhard, K. M. (1999). The
accessibility of conceptual number to the processes of subject-verb
agreement in English. Journal of Memory and
Language, 411, 560–578.
England, N. (2011). Plurality
agreement in some Eastern Mayan
languages. International Journal of American
Linguistics, 77(3), 397–412.
Foote, R., & Bock, K. (2012). The
role of morphology in subject–verb number agreement: A comparison of Mexican
and Dominican Spanish. Language and Cognitive
Processes, 27(3), 429–461.
Francis, W. N. (1986). Proximity
concord in English. Journal of English
Linguistics, 191, 309–317.
Garrett, M. F. (1980). Levels
of processing in sentence
production. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language
production (Vol. 11, pp. 177–220). London: Academic Press.
Gillespie, M., & Pearlmutter, N. (2011). Hierarchy
and scope of planning in subject-verb agreement
production. Cognition, 1181(377–397).
Haspelmath, M. (2013). Occurrence
of nominal
plurality. In M. S. Dryer & M. Haspelmath (Eds.), The
world atlas of language structures
online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology.
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The
weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and
Brain
Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83.
Hoshino, N., Dussias, P. E., & Kroll, J. F. (2010). Processing
subject-verb agreement in a second language depends on
proficiency. Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition, 13(2), 87–98.
INEGI. (2010). Censo
general de población y vivienda. Instituto Nacional de Estadística, Geografía e Informática. Mexico.
Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical
data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit
mixed models. Journal of Memory and
Language, 591, 434–446.
Jaeger, T. F., & Norcliffe, E. J. (2009). The
cross-linguistic study of sentence
production. Language and Linguistics
Compass, 31, 1–22.
Jespersen, O. (1924). The
philosophy of
grammar. London: Allen & Unwin.
Kaan, E. (2002). Investigating
the effects of distance and number interference in processing subject-verb
dependencies. Journal of Psycholinguistic
Research, 311, 165–193.
Kempen, G., & Harbusch, K. (2004). A
corpus study into word order variation in German subordinate clauses :
Animacy affects linearization independently of grammatical function
assignment. In T. Pechmann & C. Habel (Eds.), Multidisciplinary
approaches to language
production (pp. 173–181). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Kempen, G., & Hoenkamp, E. (1987). An
incremental procedural grammar for sentence
formulation. Cognitive
Science, 11(2), 201–258.
Kimball, J., & Aissen, J. (1971). I
think, you think, he think. Linguistic
Inquiry, 21, 241–246.
Kreiner, H., Garrod, S., & Sturt, P. (2013). Number
agreement in sentence comprehension: the relationship between grammatical
and conceptual factors. Language and
Cognitive
Processes, 281, 829–874.
Lanter, J. A., & Basche, R. A. (2014). Effect
of number and similarity on children’s plural
comprehension. First
Language, 341, 519–536.
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking:
From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Lorimor, H., Bock, J. K., Zalkind, E., Sheyman, A., & Beard, R. (2008). Agreement
and attraction in Russian. Language and
Cognitive
Processes, 23(6), 769–799.
Lucy, J. A. (1992). Grammatical
categories and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Nicol, J., Forster, K., & Veres, C. (1997). Subject-verb
agreement processes in comprehension. Journal
of Memory and
Language, 361, 569–587.
Nicol, J., & Greth, D. (2003). Production
of subject-verb agreement in spanish as a second
language. Experimental
Psychology, 501, 196–203.
Nicol, J., Teller, M., & Greth, D. (2001). Production
of verb agreement in monolingual, bilingual and second language
speakers. In J. Nicol (Ed.), One
mind, two languages: Billingual language
processing (pp. 117–133). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
Norcliffe, E. (2009). Head
marking in usage and grammar. A study of variation and change in Yucatec
Maya (Unpublished doctoral
dissertation). Stanford University.
Norcliffe, E., & Jaeger, T. F. (2014). Predciting
head-marking variability in Yucatec Maya relative clause
production. Language and Cognition, First
View, 1–39.
Norcliffe, E., Konopka, A. E., Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (2015). Word
order affects the time-course of sentence formulation in
Tzeltal. The cross-linguistic study of
language understanding and production, Special Issue of Language, Cognition
and
Neuroscience, 30(9), 1187–1208.
Osterhout, L., & Mobley, L. (1995). Event-related
brain potentials elicited by failure to
agree. Journal of Memory and
Language, 341, 739–773.
Pearlmutter, N. (2000). Linear
versus hierarchical agreement feature processing in
comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic
Research, 291, 89–98.
Pearlmutter, N., Garnsey, S. M., & Bock, J. K. (1999). Agreement
processes in sentence comprehension. Journal
of Memory and
Language, 411, 427–456.
Pfeiler, B. (1998). Acquisition
of number in Yucatec
Maya. In S. Gillis (Ed.), Studies
in the acquisition of number and diminutive
marking (Vol. 951, pp. 77–95). Universiteit Antwerpen: Antwerp Papers in Linguistics.
Pfeiler, B. (2001). Acerca
de la adquisición del número en el maya
yucateco. In Paper
presented at the I seminario sobre adquisición
de la lengua indígena. San Cristobal de las
Casas, Chiapas, Mexico.
Pfeiler, B. (2009). The
acquisition of numeral classifiers and optional plural marking in Yucatec
Maya. In U. Stephany & M. Voiekova (Eds.), The
acquisition of number and case from a typological perspective. Studies on
language acquisition
[SOLA] (pp. 91–110). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Pfeiler, B. (2014). Maya
and Spanish in Yucatán: An example of continuity and
change. In S. S. Mufwene (Ed.), Iberian
imerialism and language evolution in Latin
America (pp. 205–224). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Pfeiler, B., & Zámišová, L. (2006). Bilingual
education: Strategy for language maintenance or shift of Yucatec
Maya? In M. Hidalgo (Ed.), Mexican
indigenous languages at the dawn of the twenty-first
century (pp. 281–300). New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G., & Svartik, J. (1985). A
comprehensive grammar of the English
language. New York: Longman.
R Core
Team. (2016). R: A language and
environment for statistical computing (Tech.
Rep.). Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
Sauppe, S., Norcliffe, E., Konopka, A. E., Van Valin, R. D. J., & Levinson, S. C. (2013). Dependencies
first: Eye tracking evidence from sentence production in
Tagalog. In Proceedings
of the 35th annual meeting of the Cognitive
Science
Society (pp. 1265–1270). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Staub, A. (2009). On
the interpretation of the number attraction effect: Response time
evidence. Journal of Memory and
Language, 601, 308–327.
Staub, A. (2010). Response
time distributional evidence for distinct varieties of number
attraction. Cognition, 1141(447–454).
Thornton, R., & MacDonald, M. C. (2003). Plausibility
and grammatical agreement. Journal of Memory
and
Language, 481, 740–759.
Van Hell, J., & Mensies, M. (2004). Subject-verb
agreement in beginning L2 learners and fluent
bilinguals. Minneapolis, MN: Paper presented at
the 45th annual meeting of the Psychonomic
Society of America.
Vigliocco, G., Butterworth, B., & Garrett, M. F. (1996). Subject-verb
agreement in Spanish and English: differences in the role of conceptual
constraints. Cognition, 61(3), 261–98.
Vigliocco, G., Butterworth, B., & Semenza, C. (1995). Constructing
subject-verb agreement in speech: The role of semantic and morphological
factors. Journal of Memory and
Language, 341, 186–215.
Vigliocco, G., Hartsuiker, R. J., Jarema, G., & Kolk, H. H. (1996). One
or more labels on the bottles? Notional concord in Dutch and
French. Language and Cognitive
Processes, 111, 407–442.
Vigliocco, G., & Nicol, J. (1998). Separating
hierarchical relations and word order in language
production. Is proximity concord syntactic
or
linear? Cognition, 681, 13–29.
Vigliocco, G., & Zilli, T. (1999). Syntactic
accuracy in sentence production: Gender disagreement in Italian language
impaired and unimpaired speakers. Journal of
Psycholinguistic
Research, 281, 623–648.
Wagers, M. W., Lau, E. F., & Philips, C. (2009). Agreement
attraction in comprehension: Representations and
processes. Journal of Memory and
Language, 611, 206–237.
Wiltschko, M. (2008). The
syntax of non-inflectional plural
marking. Natural Language and Linguistic
Theory, 261, 639–694.
Zapf, J. A., & Smith, L. B. (2008). Meaning
matters in children’s plural
productions. Cognition, 1081, 466–476.
Cited by (1)
Cited by one other publication
Butler, Lindsay K.
2023.
Morphological and conceptual influences on the real-time comprehension of optional plural marked sentences in Yucatec Maya.
Frontiers in Psychology 14
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.