Article published In:
Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism
Vol. 8:3 (2018) ► pp.283319
References (78)
References
Acuña-Fariña, J. C. (2009). The linguistics and psycholinguistics of agreement: A tutorial overview. Lingua, 1191, 389–424. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects mixed effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 591, 390–412. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barner, D., Lui, T., & Zapf, J. (2012). Is two a plural marker in early child language? Developmental Psychology, 48(1), 10–17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015a). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, ArXiv e-print. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015b). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using eigen and s4 (R Package version 1.1–9).Google Scholar
Berg, T. (1998). The resolution of number agreement conflicts in English and German agreement patterns. Linguistics, 361, 41–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blaha Pfeiler, B., & Carrillo Carreón, C. (2001). La adquisición del maya yucateco: el número. In C. R. y Lourdes de León Pasquel (Ed.), La adquisición de la lengua materna. Espaüol, lenguas Mayas, Euskera (pp. 75–97). México: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México/Centro de Investigaciones y Estudios Superiores en Antropología Social.Google Scholar
Bock, J. K., & Cutting, J. C. (1992). Regulating mental energy: Performance units in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 311, 99–127. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K., & Eberhard, K. M. (1993). Meaning, sound and syntax in English number agreement. Language and Cognitive Processes, 81, 57–99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1994). Language production: grammatical encoding. In M. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics (pp. 945–984). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bock, J. K., & Miller, C. A. (1991). Broken agreement. Cognitive Psychology, 231, 45–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bohnemeyer, J. (2002). The grammar of time reference in Yukatek Maya. Muenchen: Lincom Europa.Google Scholar
(2009). Linking without grammatical relations in Yucatec: Alignment, extraction and control. In Y. Nishina, Y. M. Shin, S. Skopeteas, E. Verhoeven, & J. Helmbrecht (Eds.), Issues in functional-typological linguistics and language theory: A festschrift for Christian Lehmann on the occasion of his 60th birthday (pp. 185–214). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Bohnemeyer, J., Butler, L. K., & Jaeger, T. F. (2015). Head-marking and agreement: Evidence from Yucatec Maya. In J. Fleischhauer, A. Latrouite, & R. Osswald (Eds.), Explorations of the syntax-semantics interface. Düselforf: Düselforf University Press.Google Scholar
Borer, H. (2005). Structuring Sense. In Name Only (Vol. I1). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brainshark, Inc. (2014). Slideshark. [URL].
Branigan, H., Pickering, M., & Tanaka, M. (2008). Contributions of animacy to grammatical function assignment and word order during production. Lingua, 118(2), 172–189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Breslow, N. E., & Clayton, D. G. (1993). Approximate inference in generalized linear mixed models. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 88(421), 9–25.Google Scholar
Bricker, V. R. (1981). The source of the ergative split in Yucatec Maya. Journal of Mayan Linguistics, 21, 83–127.Google Scholar
Brown-Schmidt, S., & Konopka, A. E. (2008). Little houses and casas pequeüas: Message formulation and syntactic form in unscripted speech with speakers of English and Spanish. Cognition, 109(2), 274–280. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Butler, L. K. (2011). The morphosyntax and processing of number marking in Yucatec Maya (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Arizona.Google Scholar
(2012). Crosslinguistic and experimental evidence for non-Number plurals. Linguistic Variation, 12(1), 27–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Butler, L. K., Jaeger, T. F., & Bohnemeyer, J. (2014). Syntactic constraints and production preferences for optional plural marking in Yucatec Maya. In A. Machicao y Priemer, A. Nolda, & A. Sioupi (Eds.), Zwischen kern und peripherie (Studia Grammatica) (Vol. 751). Berlin: Akadmie-Verlag. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
CONAFE. (2000). Escuela y comunidades originarias en México. Consejo Nacional del Fomento Educativo. Mexico.Google Scholar
Corbett, G. G. (2000). Number. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
den Dikken, M. (2001). “Pluringulars”, pronouns and quirky agreement. The Linguistic Review, 181, 19–41.Google Scholar
Desmet, T., & Duyck, W. (2007). Bilingual language processing. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1/31, 168–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eberhard, K. M. (1999). The accessibility of conceptual number to the processes of subject-verb agreement in English. Journal of Memory and Language, 411, 560–578. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
England, N. (2011). Plurality agreement in some Eastern Mayan languages. International Journal of American Linguistics, 77(3), 397–412. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foote, R., & Bock, K. (2012). The role of morphology in subject–verb number agreement: A comparison of Mexican and Dominican Spanish. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27(3), 429–461. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Francis, W. N. (1986). Proximity concord in English. Journal of English Linguistics, 191, 309–317. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garrett, M. F. (1980). Levels of processing in sentence production. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production (Vol. 11, pp. 177–220). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gillespie, M., & Pearlmutter, N. (2011). Hierarchy and scope of planning in subject-verb agreement production. Cognition, 1181(377–397). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haspelmath, M. (2013). Occurrence of nominal plurality. In M. S. Dryer & M. Haspelmath (Eds.), The world atlas of language structures online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoshino, N., Dussias, P. E., & Kroll, J. F. (2010). Processing subject-verb agreement in a second language depends on proficiency. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(2), 87–98. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
INEGI. (2010). Censo general de población y vivienda. Instituto Nacional de Estadística, Geografía e Informática. Mexico.Google Scholar
Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 591, 434–446. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jaeger, T. F., & Norcliffe, E. J. (2009). The cross-linguistic study of sentence production. Language and Linguistics Compass, 31, 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jespersen, O. (1924). The philosophy of grammar. London: Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Kaan, E. (2002). Investigating the effects of distance and number interference in processing subject-verb dependencies. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 311, 165–193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kempen, G., & Harbusch, K. (2004). A corpus study into word order variation in German subordinate clauses : Animacy affects linearization independently of grammatical function assignment. In T. Pechmann & C. Habel (Eds.), Multidisciplinary approaches to language production (pp. 173–181). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kempen, G., & Hoenkamp, E. (1987). An incremental procedural grammar for sentence formulation. Cognitive Science, 11(2), 201–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kimball, J., & Aissen, J. (1971). I think, you think, he think. Linguistic Inquiry, 21, 241–246.Google Scholar
Kreiner, H., Garrod, S., & Sturt, P. (2013). Number agreement in sentence comprehension: the relationship between grammatical and conceptual factors. Language and Cognitive Processes, 281, 829–874. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lanter, J. A., & Basche, R. A. (2014). Effect of number and similarity on children’s plural comprehension. First Language, 341, 519–536. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lorimor, H., Bock, J. K., Zalkind, E., Sheyman, A., & Beard, R. (2008). Agreement and attraction in Russian. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(6), 769–799. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lucy, J. A. (1992). Grammatical categories and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicol, J., Forster, K., & Veres, C. (1997). Subject-verb agreement processes in comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 361, 569–587. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicol, J., & Greth, D. (2003). Production of subject-verb agreement in spanish as a second language. Experimental Psychology, 501, 196–203. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicol, J., Teller, M., & Greth, D. (2001). Production of verb agreement in monolingual, bilingual and second language speakers. In J. Nicol (Ed.), One mind, two languages: Billingual language processing (pp. 117–133). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Norcliffe, E. (2009). Head marking in usage and grammar. A study of variation and change in Yucatec Maya (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Stanford University.Google Scholar
Norcliffe, E., & Jaeger, T. F. (2014). Predciting head-marking variability in Yucatec Maya relative clause production. Language and Cognition, First View, 1–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norcliffe, E., Konopka, A. E., Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (2015). Word order affects the time-course of sentence formulation in Tzeltal. The cross-linguistic study of language understanding and production, Special Issue of Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30(9), 1187–1208.Google Scholar
Osterhout, L., & Mobley, L. (1995). Event-related brain potentials elicited by failure to agree. Journal of Memory and Language, 341, 739–773. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pearlmutter, N. (2000). Linear versus hierarchical agreement feature processing in comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 291, 89–98. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pearlmutter, N., Garnsey, S. M., & Bock, J. K. (1999). Agreement processes in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 411, 427–456. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pfeiler, B. (1998). Acquisition of number in Yucatec Maya. In S. Gillis (Ed.), Studies in the acquisition of number and diminutive marking (Vol. 951, pp. 77–95). Universiteit Antwerpen: Antwerp Papers in Linguistics.Google Scholar
(2001). Acerca de la adquisición del número en el maya yucateco. In Paper presented at the I seminario sobre adquisición de la lengua indígena. San Cristobal de las Casas, Chiapas, Mexico.
(2009). The acquisition of numeral classifiers and optional plural marking in Yucatec Maya. In U. Stephany & M. Voiekova (Eds.), The acquisition of number and case from a typological perspective. Studies on language acquisition [SOLA] (pp. 91–110). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
(2014). Maya and Spanish in Yucatán: An example of continuity and change. In S. S. Mufwene (Ed.), Iberian imerialism and language evolution in Latin America (pp. 205–224). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pfeiler, B., & Zámišová, L. (2006). Bilingual education: Strategy for language maintenance or shift of Yucatec Maya? In M. Hidalgo (Ed.), Mexican indigenous languages at the dawn of the twenty-first century (pp. 281–300). New York: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G., & Svartik, J. (1985). A comprehensive grammar of the English language. New York: Longman.Google Scholar
R Core Team. (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing (Tech. Rep.). Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Sauppe, S., Norcliffe, E., Konopka, A. E., Van Valin, R. D. J., & Levinson, S. C. (2013). Dependencies first: Eye tracking evidence from sentence production in Tagalog. In Proceedings of the 35th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1265–1270). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Staub, A. (2009). On the interpretation of the number attraction effect: Response time evidence. Journal of Memory and Language, 601, 308–327. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010). Response time distributional evidence for distinct varieties of number attraction. Cognition, 1141(447–454). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thornton, R., & MacDonald, M. C. (2003). Plausibility and grammatical agreement. Journal of Memory and Language, 481, 740–759. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Hell, J., & Mensies, M. (2004). Subject-verb agreement in beginning L2 learners and fluent bilinguals. Minneapolis, MN: Paper presented at the 45th annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society of America. DOI logo
Vigliocco, G., Butterworth, B., & Garrett, M. F. (1996). Subject-verb agreement in Spanish and English: differences in the role of conceptual constraints. Cognition, 61(3), 261–98. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vigliocco, G., Butterworth, B., & Semenza, C. (1995). Constructing subject-verb agreement in speech: The role of semantic and morphological factors. Journal of Memory and Language, 341, 186–215. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vigliocco, G., Hartsuiker, R. J., Jarema, G., & Kolk, H. H. (1996). One or more labels on the bottles? Notional concord in Dutch and French. Language and Cognitive Processes, 111, 407–442. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vigliocco, G., & Nicol, J. (1998). Separating hierarchical relations and word order in language production. Is proximity concord syntactic or linear? Cognition, 681, 13–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vigliocco, G., & Zilli, T. (1999). Syntactic accuracy in sentence production: Gender disagreement in Italian language impaired and unimpaired speakers. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 281, 623–648. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wagers, M. W., Lau, E. F., & Philips, C. (2009). Agreement attraction in comprehension: Representations and processes. Journal of Memory and Language, 611, 206–237. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wiltschko, M. (2008). The syntax of non-inflectional plural marking. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 261, 639–694. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zapf, J. A., & Smith, L. B. (2008). Meaning matters in children’s plural productions. Cognition, 1081, 466–476. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (1)

Cited by one other publication

Butler, Lindsay K.
2023. Morphological and conceptual influences on the real-time comprehension of optional plural marked sentences in Yucatec Maya. Frontiers in Psychology 14 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.