Article published In:
Representation and Processing in Bilingual Morphology
Edited by Jennifer R. Austin
[Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 9:1] 2019
► pp. 641
References
Babcock, L., Stowe, J. C., Maloof, C. J., Brovetto, C., & Ullman, M. T.
(2012) The storage and composition of inflected forms in adult-learned second language: A study of the influence of length of residence, age of arrival, sex, and other factors. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 151, 820–840. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Basnight-Brown, D. M., Chen, L., Hua, S., Kostić, A., & Feldman, L. B.
(2007) Monolingual and bilingual recognition of regular and irregular English verbs: Sensitivity to form similarity varies with first language experience. Journal of Memory and Language, 571, 65–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Devescovi, A., Hernandez, A., & Pizzamiglio, L.
(1996) Gender priming in Italian. Perception & Psychophysics, 581, 992–1004. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S.
(2014) lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1-7, <URL: [URL].
Bowden, H. W., Gelfand, M. P., Sanz, C., & Ullman, M. T.
(2010) Verbal inflectional morphology in L1 and L2 Spanish: A frequency effects study examining storage versus composition. Language Learning, 601, 44–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., Balkhair, L., Schutter, J-S., & Cunnings, I.
(2013) The time course of morphological processing in a second language. Second Language Research, 291, 7–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C.
(2006) Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 271, 3–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., Felser, C., Neubauer, K., Sato, M., & Silva, R.
(2010) Morphological structure in native and nonnative language processing. Language Learning, 601, 21–43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coughlin, C. E., & Tremblay, A.
(2013) Proficiency and working memory based explanations for nonnative speakers’ sensitivity to agreement in sentence processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 341, 647–647. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) Morphological decomposition in native and non-native French speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 181, 524–542. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diependaele, K., Lemhöfer, K., & Brysbaert, M.
(2013) The word frequency effect in first- and second-language word recognition: A lexical entrenchment account. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 661, 843–863, DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crepaldi, D., Rastle, K., Coltheart, M., & Nickels, L.
(2010) ‘Fell’ primes ‘fall’, but does ‘bell’ prime ‘ball’? Masked priming with irregularly-inflected primes. Journal of Memory and Language, 631, 83–99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P. E., Valdés Kroff, J. R., Guzzardo Tamargo, R. E., & Gerfen, C.
(2013) When gender and looking go hand in hand: Grammatical gender processing in L2 Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 351, 353–387. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Feldman, L. B., Kostić, A., Basnight-Brown, D. M., Filipović Đurđević, D., Pastizzo, M. J.
(2010) Morphological facilitation for regular and irregular verb formations in native and non-native speakers: Little evidence for two distinct mechanisms. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 131, 119–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foote, R.
(2015a) The production of gender agreement in native and L2 Spanish: The role of morphophonological form. Second Language Research, 31(3), 343–373. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015b) The storage and processing of morphologically complex words in L2 Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition. FirstView Article DOI logo Published online: 11 December 2015Google Scholar
Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C.
(2003) DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods Instruments and Computers, 35.11, 116–124. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foucart, A., Martin, C. D., Moreno, E. M., & Costa, A.
(2014) Can bilinguals see it coming? Word anticipation in L2 sentence reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 401, 1461–1469. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldschneider, J. M., & DeKeyser, R. M.
(2001) Explaining the “Natural Order of L2 Morpheme Acquisition” in English: A Meta-analysis of multiple determinants. Language Learning, 511, 1–50.Google Scholar
Gor, K.
(2015) Phonology and morphology in lexical processing. In: J. W. Schwieter (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Bilingual Processing (pp. 173–199). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gor, K., Chrabaszcz, A., & Cook, S.
(2017) Processing of nonnative inflected words: Beyond affix stripping. Journal of Memory and Language, 931, 315–332. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gor, K., & Cook, S.
(2010) Non-native processing of verbal morphology: In search of regularity. Language Learning, 601, 88–126. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gor, K., & Jackson, S.
(2013) Morphological decomposition and lexical access in a native and second language: A nesting doll effect. Language and Cognitive Processes, 281, 1065–1091. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gurjanov, M., Lukatela, G., Lukatela, K., Savić, M., & Turvey, M. T.
(1985) Grammatical priming of inflected nouns by the gender of possessive adjectives. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 111, 692.Google Scholar
Gurjanov, M., Lukatela, G., Moskovljević, J., Savić, M., & Turvey, M. T.
(1985) Grammatical priming of inflected nouns by inflected adjectives. Cognition, 191, 55–71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H.
(2010) Ultimate attainment in L2 inflection: Performance similarities between nonnative and native speakers. Lingua, 1201, 901–931. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) Grammatical gender in adult L2 acquisition: Relations between lexical and syntactic variability. Second Language Research, 291, 33–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H., & León Arriaga, M. E.
(2016) Structural and inherent case in the non-native processing of Spanish: Constraints on inflectional variability. Second Language Research, 321, 75–108. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jakobson, R.
(1984) Contributions to the general theory of case: General meanings of the Russian cases. Russian and Slavic Grammar: Studies, 1931–1981, pp. 59–103. Berlin-New York: Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jegerski, J.
(2016) Number attraction effects in near-native Spanish sentence comprehension. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 381, 5–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jiang, N.
(2004) Morphological insensitivity in second language processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 251, 603–634. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Selective integration of linguistic knowledge in adult second language learning. Language Learning, 571, 1–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jiang, N., Hu, G., Chrabaszcz, A., & Ye, L.
(2017) The activation of grammaticalized meaning in L2 processing: Toward an explanation of the morphological congruency effect. International Journal of Bilingualism. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kerek, E., & Niemi, P.
(2009) Russian orthography and learning to read. Reading in a Foreign Language, 211, 1–21. [URL]
Kirkici, B., & Clahsen, H.
(2013) Inflection and derivation in native and non-native language processing: Masked priming experiments on Turkish. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 161, 776–791. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P., & Christensen, R.
(2015) lmerTest: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 2.0–25. [URL].
Lukatela, G., Gligorijevic, B., Kostić, A., & Turvey, M. T.
(1980) Representation of inflected nouns in the internal lexicon. Memory and Cognition, 81, 415–423. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lukatela, G., Kostić, A., Feldman, L. B., & Turvey, M. T.
(1983) Grammatical priming of inflected nouns. Memory & Cognition, 111, 59–63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lukatela, G., Moraca, J., Stojnov, D., Savic, M. D., Katz, L., & Turvey, M. T.
(1982) Grammatical priming effects between pronouns and inflected verb forms. Psychological Research, 441, 297–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Milin, P., Filipović Đurđević, D., Moscoso del Prado Martin, F.
(2009) The simultaneous effects of inflectional paradigms and classes on lexical recognition: Evidence from Serbian. Journal of Memory and Language, 601, 50–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., Foote, R., & Perpiñán, S.
(2008) Gender agreement in adult second language learners and Spanish heritage speakers: The effects of age and context of acquisition. Language Learning, 581, 503–553. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Neubauer, K., & Clahsen, H.
(2009) Decomposition of inflected words in a second language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 311, 403–435. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Piper, T. H., & Leicester, P. F.
(1980) Word association behavior as an indicator of English language proficiency, Educational Resources Information Center (ERIC) documents [online]. Available: Accession number ED 227651.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M.
(2008) Heritage language narratives. In D. Brinton, O. Kagan, & S. Bauckus (Eds.), Heritage language education: A new field emerging (pp. 108–156). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Portin, M., Lehtonen, M., Harrer, G., Wande, E., Niemi, J., & Laine, M.
(2008) L1 effects on the processing of inflected nouns in L2. Acta Psychologica, 1281, 452–465. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Portin, M., Lehtonen, M., & Laine, M.
(2007) Processing of inflected nouns in late bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 281, 135–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Core Team
(2015) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL [URL].
Rastle, K., & Davis, M. H.
(2008) Morphological decomposition based on the analysis of orthography. Language and Cognitive Processes, 231, 942–971. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reips, U. D.
(2002) Standards for Internet-based experimenting. Experimental psychology, 49(4), 243. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Romanova, N., & Gor, K.
(2017) Processing gender and number agreement in Russian as a second language: The devil is in the details. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 391, 97–128. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rubinstein, G.
(1995) On acquisition of Russian cases by American classroom learners. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 331, 9–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Samojlova, M., & Slioussar, N.
(2014) Frequencies of different grammatical features and inflectional affixes of Russian nouns: a database. Retrieved from [URL]
Schoeffler, M., Stöter, F. R., Bayerlein, H., Edler, B., & Herre, J.
(2013) An experiment about estimating the number of instruments in polyphonic music: A comparison between internet and laboratory results. ISMIR, 389–394.Google Scholar
Scontras, G., Fuchs, Z., & Polinsky, M.
(2015) Heritage language and linguistic theory. Frontiers in Psychology, 6.15451. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shcherba, L. V.
(1957) The main principles of orthography and their social significance. In: Selected Works on Russian Language, pp. 45–49. Moscow: Uchpedgiz.Google Scholar
Silva, R., & Clahsen, H.
(2008) Morphologically complex words in L1 and L2 processing: Evidence from masked priming experiments in English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 111, 245–260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slioussar, N., Stetsenko, A., & Matyushkina, T.
(2017) Producing case errors in Russian. In Y. Oseki, M. Esipova, and S. Harves (Eds.), Formal Approaches to Slavic Linguistics: The First New York Meeting 2015. (pp. 363–379). Ann Arbor, Michigan: Michigan Slavic Publications.Google Scholar
Taft, M.
(2004) Morphological decomposition and the reverse base frequency effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 571, 745–765. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vainio, S., Pajunen, A., & Hyönä, J.
(2015) Processing modifier – head agreement in L1 and L2 Finnish: An eye-tracking study. Second Language Research, 321, 3–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Voga, М., Anastassiadis-Symeonidis, А., & Giraudo, H.
Witzel, J., Cornelius, S., Witzel, N., Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C.
(2013) Testing the viability of webDMDX for masked priming experiments. The Mental Lexicon, 81, 421–449. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wolter, B.
(2001) Comparing the L1 and L2 mental lexicon. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 231, 41–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 5 other publications

Austin, Jennifer
2019. Representation and processing in bilingual morphology. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 9:1  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Cherepovskaia, Natalia, Elizaveta Reutova & Natalia Slioussar
2021. Becoming Native-Like for Good or Ill: Online and Offline Processing of Case Forms in L2 Russian. Frontiers in Psychology 12 DOI logo
Cherepovskaia, Natalia, Natalia Slioussar & Anna Denissenko Denissenko
2021. Acquisition of the nominal case system in Russian as a second language. Second Language Research  pp. 026765832098805 ff. DOI logo
Gor, Kira, Anna Chrabaszcz & Svetlana Cook
Uygun, Serkan & Harald Clahsen
2021. Morphological processing in heritage speakers: A masked priming study on the Turkish aorist. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 24:3  pp. 415 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 23 april 2022. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.