Article published in:
Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism
Vol. 10:1 (2020) ► pp. 109151
References
Alario, F. -X., Perre, L., Castel, C., & Ziegler, J. C.
(2007) The role of orthography in speech production revisited. Cognition, 102(3), 464–475. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S.
(2013) lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.0-5. http://​CRAN​.R​-project​.org​/package​=lme4
Bock, K.
(1991) A sketchbook of production problems. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Special Issue: Sentence Processing, 201, 141–160. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chen, J. -Y.
(1993) A small corpus of speech errors in Mandarin Chinese and their classification. World of Chinese Language, 691, 26–41.Google Scholar
Chen, J. -Y., Chen, T. -M., & Dell, G. S.
(2002) Word-Form Encoding in Mandarin Chinese as Assessed by the Implicit Priming Task. Journal of Memory and Language, 46(4), 751–781. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chen, J. -Y., & Li, C. -Y.
(2011) Word form encoding in Chinese word naming and word typing. Cognition, 121(1), 140–146. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chen, T. -M., & Chen, J. -Y.
(2013) The syllable as the proximate unit in Mandarin Chinese word production: An intrinsic or accidental property of the production system? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(1), 154–162. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chen, J. Y., Lin, W. C., & Ferrand, L.
(2003) Masked priming of the syllable in Mandarin Chinese speech production. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 45(1), 107–120.Google Scholar
Cheng, C. -C.
(1973) A synchronic phonology of Mandarin Chinese. The Hague: Mouton CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Damian, M. F., & Dumay, N.
(2007) Time pressure and phonological advance planning in spoken production. Journal of Memory and Language, 571, 195–209. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009) Exploring phonological encoding through repeated segments. Language and Cognitive Processes, 241, 685–712. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dupoux, E., Kakehi, K., Hirose, Y., Pallier, C., & Mehler, J.
(1999) Epenthetic vowels in Japanese: A perceptual illusion? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(6), 1568–1581.Google Scholar
Durgunoğlu, A. Y., Nagy, W. E., & Hancin-Bhatt, B. J.
(1993) Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(3), 453–465. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C.
(2003) DMDX: A windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 351, 116–124. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goldinger, S. D., & Azuma, T.
(2003) Puzzle-solving science: The quixotic quest for units in speech perception. Journal of Phonetics, 31(3), 305–320. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gottardo, A., Yan, B., Siegel, L. S., & Wade-Woolley, L.
(2001) Factors related to English reading performance in children with Chinese as a first language: More evidence of cross-language transfer of phonological processing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(3), 530–542. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grotjahn, R., Klein-Braley, C., & Raatz, U.
(2002) C-Test: an over-view. In J. A. Coleman, R. Grotjahn & U. Raatz (Eds.), University Language Testing and the C-Test. (pp. 93–114). Bochum: AKS-Verlag Finkenstaedt.Google Scholar
Hyman, L. M.
(1985) A theory of phonological weight. Dordrecht: Foris. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ida, K., Nakayama, M., & Lupker, S. J.
(2015) The functional phonological unit of Japanese-English bilinguals is language dependent: Evidence from masked onset and mora priming effects. Japanese Psychological Research, 57(1), 38–49. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, C. L. & Dell, G. S.
(2014) ‘hotdog’ not ‘hot’ ‘dog’: The phonological planning of compound words. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience, 291, 512–523. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kanopka, A. E.
(2012) Planning ahead: How recent experience with structures and words changes the scope of linguistic planning. Journal of Memory and Language, 401, 153–194.Google Scholar
Kinoshita, S.
(2000) The left-to-right nature of the masked onset priming effect in naming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 71, 133–141. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kubozono, H.
(1989).The mora and syllable structure in Japanese: Evidence from speech errors. Language and Speech, 32(3), 249–278. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kureta, Y., Fushimi, T., Sakuma, N., & Tatsumi, I. F.
(2015) Orthographic influences on the word-onset phoneme preparation effect in native Japanese speakers: Evidence from the word-form preparation paradigm. Japanese Psychological Research, 57(1), 50–60. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kureta, Y., Fushimi, T., & Tatsumi, I. F.
(2006) The functional unit in phonological encoding: Evidence for moraic representation in native Japanese speakers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(5), 1102–1119. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B.
(2015) lmerTest: tests in linear mixed effects models. R package version 2.0-29. http://​CRAN​.R​-project​.org​/package​=lmerTest
Levelt, W. J. M.
(1989) Speaking: From intention to articulation. Bradford, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J.
(1992) Accessing words in speech production: Stages, processes and representations. Cognition, 42(1), 1–22. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W. J., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S.
(1999) A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(01), 1–38. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Li, C., Wang, M., & Davis, J. A.
(2017) The phonological preparation unit in spoken word production in a second language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20 (2) 2017, 351–366. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Li, C., Wang, M., & Idsardi, W.
(2015) The effect of orthographic form-cuing on the phonological preparation unit in spoken word production. Memory & Cognition, 43(4), 563–578. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M.
(2007) The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 940–967. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, A. S.
(1990) The time course of phonological encoding in language production: The encoding of successive syllables of a word. Journal of Memory and Language, 29(5), 524–545. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Bialystok, E., Chong, K. K., & Li, Y.
(2004) Levels of phonological awareness in three cultures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89(2), 93–111. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, A. S.
(1991) The time course of phonological encoding in language production: Phonological encoding inside a syllable. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(1), 69–89. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nakayama, M., Kinoshita, S., & Verdonschot, R. G.
(2016) The Emergence of a Phoneme-Sized Unit in L2 Speech Production: Evidence from Japanese–English Bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 71: 175. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
O’Seaghdha, P. G., Chen, J. -Y., & Chen, T. -M.
(2010) Proximate units in word production: Phonological encoding begins with syllables in Mandarin Chinese but with segments in English. Cognition, 115(2), 282–302. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
O’Séaghdha, P. G., & Frazer, A. K.
(2014) The exception does not rule: Attention constrains form preparation in word production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(3), 797–810. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Otake, T., Hatano, G., Cutler, A., & Mehler, J.
(1993) Mora or syllable? Speech segmentation in Japanese. Journal of memory and language, 32(2), 258–278. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
R Development Core Team
(2008) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. 3-900051-07-0, URL http://​www​.R​-project​.org.
Roelofs, A.
(1999) Phonological segments and features as planning units in speech production. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(2), 173–200. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S.
(1998) Metrical structure in planning the production of spoken words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(4), 922–939. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Shu, H., Peng, H., & McBride-Chang, C.
(2008) Phonological awareness in young Chinese children. Developmental Science, 11(1), 171–181. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tseng, C. H., Huang, K. Y., & Jeng, J. Y.
(1996) The role of the syllable in perceiving spoken Chinese. Proceedings of the National Science Council, 6(1), 7l–86Google Scholar
Van de Velde, M., Meyer, A. S., & Konopka, A. E.
(2014) Message formulation and structural assembly: describing “easy” and “hard” events with preferred and dispreferred syntactic structures. Journal of Memory and Language, 71(1), 124–144. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Verdonschot, R. G., Kiyama, S., Tamaoka, K., Kinoshita, S., La Heij, W., & Schiller, N. O.
(2011) The functional unit of Japanese word naming: evidence from masked priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(6), 1458–1473.Google Scholar
Verdonschot, R. G., Lai, J., Chen, F., Tamaoka, K., & Schiller, N. O.
(2015) Constructing initial phonology in Mandarin Chinese: Syllabic or subsyllabic? A masked priming investigation. Japanese Psychological Research, 57(1), 61–68. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Verdonschot, R. G., Nakayama, M., Zhang, Q., Tamaoka, K., & Schiller, N. O.
(2013) The Proximate Phonological Unit of Chinese-English Bilinguals: Proficiency Matters. PLoS ONE, 8(4), e61454. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, V., Jescheniak, J. D., & Schriefers, H.
(2010) On the flexibility of grammatical advance planning during sentence production: Effects of cognitive load on multiple lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(2), 423–440.Google Scholar
Wong, A. W. -K., & Chen, H. -C.
(2008) Processing segmental and prosodic information in Cantonese word production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 341, 1172–1190.Google Scholar
(2009) What are effective phonological units in Cantonese spoken word planning? Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 161, 888–892. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
You, W., Zhang, Q., & Verdonschot, R. G.
(2012) Masked syllable priming effects in word and picture naming in Chinese. PloS One, 7(10), e46595. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Li, Chuchu, Min Wang, Say Young Kim, Donald J. Bolger & Kelly Wright
2022. Phonological Preparation in Korean: Phoneme, or Syllable or Another Unit?. Language and Speech 65:2  pp. 337 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 23 april 2022. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.